Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 Scientist should improve the understanding of detection dogs olfaction. They should also develop the tests to select and assess the performance of detection dogs. the formulation of a aids mimicking the smell of cadavers could increase dog performances + 1878 word(s) 1878 2020-07-23 10:06:59 |
2 update layout and reference -1 word(s) 1877 2020-07-30 04:30:37 | |
3 Increase the exposure -15 word(s) 1862 2020-10-26 09:35:47 |

Video Upload Options

We provide professional Video Production Services to translate complex research into visually appealing presentations. Would you like to try it?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Martin, C.; Diederich, C.; Verheggen, F. Human Remains Detection Dogs. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/1464 (accessed on 21 December 2024).
Martin C, Diederich C, Verheggen F. Human Remains Detection Dogs. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/1464. Accessed December 21, 2024.
Martin, Clément, Claire Diederich, François Verheggen. "Human Remains Detection Dogs" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/1464 (accessed December 21, 2024).
Martin, C., Diederich, C., & Verheggen, F. (2020, July 29). Human Remains Detection Dogs. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/1464
Martin, Clément, et al. "Human Remains Detection Dogs." Encyclopedia. Web. 29 July, 2020.
Human Remains Detection Dogs
Edit

Human remains detection dogs (HRDDs) are powerful police assets to locate a corpse. However, methods used to select and train them are as diverse as the number of countries with such a canine brigade. First, a survey was sent to human remains searching brigades (N Countries = 10; N Brigades = 16; N Handlers = 50; N questions = 9), to collect their working habits. It confirmed the lack of optimized selection and training procedures. Second, a literature review was performed in order to outline the strengths and shortcomings of HRDDs training. A comparison between the scientific knowledge and common practices used by HRDDs brigade was then conducted focusing on HRDDs selection and training procedures. We highlighted that HRDD handlers explained to select their dogs by focusing on behavioral traits while neglecting anatomical features, which have shown their importance. Most HRDD handlers reported to use a reward-based training, which is in accordance with training literature for dogs. Olfactory training aids should be representative of the odor target to allow a dog to reach optimal performances. The survey highlighted the wide diversity of homemade olfactory training aids, and the need to optimize their composition. In the present document, key research topics to improve HRDD works are also provided.

Human remains detection dogs dog behavior dog selection dog training forensic science

1. Definition

Human remains detection dogs (HRDDs), also called cadaver dogs,  are powerful police assets to locate decaying corpses. They have been already defined by A. Rebmann et al as “canines specially trained to find human decomposition scent and alert their handler to its location” [1]. They are used by law enforcements in many contexts including homicides, as well as natural and manmade disasters to search for human cadavers, body parts, or fluids [1][2][3][4][5][6][7]. Operations during which HRDDS are used are gathered under the term necrosearch[8].

2. Introduction

During dog domestication, humans have selected individuals upon their olfactory abilities, which were particularly important to the development of hunting dog breeds [9]. As a result, today’s breeds of working dogs show accurate, sensitive, and reliable olfactory abilities [6][10][11][12][13][14]. Dogs are able to recognize several hundred compounds due to a wide diversity of olfactory receptor cells present in their olfactory epithelium[15][13][14][16][17]. In addition, polymorphism in olfactory receptors genes increases the number of molecules that can be bound, making dogs powerful biological detectors. Not all dogs are suitable to perform detection work, due to e.g., their head conformation, their olfactory sensibility, or the polymorphism of their olfactory receptor genes [15][8][12][13][14][18][19].

The diversify of the HRDDs' working environments requires dogs with specific skills. Handlers would benefit from identifying morphological, olfactory, and behavioral traits that indicate a dog’s suitability to become a HRDD. However, poor information is available regarding the selection of HRDDs, including those traits and their procedures as the scientific literature mainly focuses on the selection of service dogs [20][21].

The training of detection dogs relies on the quality of the available olfactory training aids[22]. HRDDs should be trained with aids that mimic the smell of any human cadaver. However, characterization of the volatile organic compounds released by human corpses are scarce as they are complicated to carry out [23]. Some training aids are available on the market but were shown to be unrepresentative of the smell of a real decaying corpse, which seems logical as regard to the lack of information thereabout[24]. Numerous biotic (e.g., tissues nature, animal species, necrophagous insects, microorganisms) and abiotic factors (e.g., humidity, temperature, death location) affect the volatilome of a cadaver [25][26][27][28][29].  As a result, the volatile chemical cues released during the decomposition process differ greatly from one case to another [4][25][27][29][30], making it difficult to provide HRDDs’ handlers with olfactory training aids mimicking human decomposition.

3. Selection and Training of Human Remains Detection Dogs

Selection - Based on a survey sent to 50 handlers from 16 brigades affiliated to Kynopol, no standardized procedures to select dogs within a litter were highlighted. All teams (except one) declared to implement homemade selection tests to assess the learning abilities of puppies during preliminary training sessions, taking into account some specific traits (e.g., drive, independence). In most cases, the selection is based on the handler’s own opinion during training or playing time only, without collecting any quantitative measures (“gut” feeling). The survey highlighted that handlers usually select puppies within the same breed (Figure 1) but none of them declared to select their dogs based on olfactory traits and they were almost entirely unaware of any morphological traits. However, the scientific literature mentioned some morphological, olfactory and behavioral traits as very important to take into account during the selection of future HRDDs. Regarding morphological traits, handlers should select agile dogs to move easily in rough fields with a steady stamina for long-lasting performances[1][31]. In addition, the body size and the fur length should be taken into account as they were shown to be directly linked to the dogs thermoregulation[19][32][33][34]. Regarding olfactory traits, it was demonstrated that the dog's head conformation was directly linked to its olfaction efficiency[35][36]. Finally, the behavioral traits that were highlighted by previous researches to be important are a high dog drives to ease the training and [18][37][38] (handlers declared to look for this trait too) and a high nerve strength to allow the dog to deal with stressful situations[18][37]. No selection procedures were found in the literature and even if several selection procedures might be applied to HRDDs, different works require different skills. We therefore strongly recommend to develop specific HRDDs' selection procedure based on abovementioned traits.  

Figure 1. Proportion of breeds currently used as human remains detection dogs based on a survey completed by ten brigades from Europe and Canada.

Training - During their training sessions, most surveyed handlers declared using reinforcement rather than punishment (Figure 2). A minority of them, however, still mentioned the practice of punishments (Figure 2). Among handlers practicing positive reinforcement, no generalization can be made. They reported to use either toys, clicker, food, or encouragement as positive stimuli. Most of them use a combination of those rewards. Handlers did not explain how their training sessions are organized. No information was provided about the procedure to end a training session.

Figure 2. Types of operant conditioning used by the surveyed handlers (y axes, N = 50) to train their human remains detection dogs with the pie chart representing the proportion of reinforcement and punishment technics used (light grey: punishment, dark grey: reinforcement).

Stimuli, also called aids, used during the specialized olfactory training were very different among surveyed teams (Figure 3). The latter reported to only use homemade biological aids (mainly of human origin or swine origin). Commercial olfactory aids available are not used by surveyed handlers.

Figure 3. Types of training aids used by the surveyed human remains detection dogs’ brigades to train their dogs. Colors in the pie charts represent the diversity of training aids used by the teams in the country where the chart is located. The bar chart represents the number of human remains detection dogs’ teams (y axe) which use the different training aids. (N Countries = 10; N Brigades = 16; N Handlers = 50; N questions = 9)

Training — Documented HRDDs’ training methods were scarce. When available, they focused on case studies[11] and none of them were associated with an evaluation of the dog's performances and behavioral outcomes. Training methods were based on operant conditioning during which a dog learns that its response to a command has consequences. The dog will repeat the behavioral response to the specific stimulus that provides the higher benefit [39][40]. A positive reinforcement strategy allowed the establishment of a strong dog-handler relationship, while negative reinforcement was associated with distraction and lower obedience [39]. Despite these observations, the type of reinforcement remains a matter of debate among surveyed handlers: it is still unclear whether aversive and reward-based methods led to different levels of performance in working dogs [41]. Training methods used to reinforce HRDDs are important. However, the the end-of-training procedure was seen to be at least as important as the training procedure itself. In a recent publication [42], the Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI) raised the importance of End of Session Cues (EoSC). EoSC is a (series of) stimulus that informs the dog about the coming end of the training session. EoSC must be clearly and repeatedly introduced at the end of each training session to avoid the dog to associate events that follow the training session (e.g., return in the cage) as part of the training. If EoSC is not applied, some dog's behavioral responses might unintentionally be reinforced. For instance, going back to the crate immediately after a training session could be considered by the dog as a punishment, resulting in a decrease in its performances. Instead a training session should end in a positive way, for example by a playing period[42][43]. There is a wide diversity of olfactory materials to train HRDDs, including biological and synthetic aids [23]. As far as the authors know, no publication comparing the efficiency of various olfactory training aids on dogs’ performances is available. However, based on chemical analyses, scientists seem to agree that the biological olfactory aids are the most efficient and reliable ones to train HRDDs [4][44], but difficult to obtain due to ethics, legislation, and biohazard risk for both human and dogs [5][23]. Commercial aids, also called synthetics aids, were identified as unrepresentative and unreliable, because they do not contain compounds that have been previously reported within the headspace of human decaying bodies [23].

Our survey revealed that handlers mostly use human origin aids which is in accordance with the scientific recommendations. However, they also meet biohazard and ethical problems. Those who use a human surrogate model to train their dogs have to face to the potential differences existing between these models and human corpses. None of the handlers mentioned the use of commercial aids. In our opinion, developing a reliable synthetic aid is a promising perspective that would ease the training efforts of HRDD brigades. It requires a fine characterization of human post-mortem volatilome, and the identification of its key components, common to any decomposing corpse [24].

4. Conclusion 

In addition to the recommendations made above, we suggest some perspectives of research and suggestions to the HRDD brigades.

First, there is a lack of validated methods regarding the selection of HRDDs puppies. We suggest to validate the existing methods of selection and to develop a new selection method based on anatomical, olfactory, and behavioral traits. As surveyed handlers declared not to record quantifiable data during the selection, this new method should include measurable data to avoid subjectivity and to select the most promising individuals. So far, no biomarkers indicating a potential good detection dog are available. One should therefore investigate the possibility to use, during the dog selection, biomarkers associated with high dog’s performances. To validate methods or potential biomarkers, it is important to develop a protocol for measuring dog performances. 

Regarding training procedures, one should compare the impact of conditioning methods (including clicker training) and the type of olfactory aids on HRDDs’ performances. The impact of the composition of a HRDD training aid (biological or synthetic) on the dog performance has never been evaluated. In our opinion, the development of a synthetic aid mimicking the human cadaver volatilome is likely one of the most promising perspectives, as this would provide handlers with a training tool that is reliable, ethical, easy to obtain and to use.

References

  1. Rebmann, A.; David, E.; Sorg, M.. Cadaver Dog Handbook; Press, CRC, Eds.; Coope, Judith: New york, 2000; pp. 196.
  2. Komar, D. The Use of Cadaver Dogs in Locating Scattered, Scavenged Human Remains: Preliminary Field Test Results. J. Forensic Sci. 1999, 44, 405–408.
  3. Migala, A.F.; Brown, S.E. Use of human remains detection dogs for wide area search after wildfire: A new experience for texas task force 1 search and rescue resources. Wilderness Environ. Med. 2012, 23, 337–342.
  4. Nizio, K.D.; Ueland, M.; Stuart, B.H.; Forbes, S.L. The analysis of textiles associated with decomposing remains as a natural training aid for cadaver-detection dogs. Forensic Chem. 2017, 5, 33–45
  5. Oesterhelweg, L.; Kröber, S.; Rottmann, K.; Willhöft, J.; Braun, C.; Thies, N.; Püschel, K.; Silkenath, J.; Gehl, A. Cadaver dogs—A study on detection of contaminated carpet squares. Forensic Sci. Int. 2008, 174, 35–39.
  6. Riezzo, I.; Neri, M.; Rendine, M.; Bellifemina, A.; Cantatore, S.; Fiore, C.; Turillazzi, E. Cadaver dogs: Unscientific myth or reliable biological devices? Forensic Sci. Int. 2014, 244, 213–221.
  7. Rust, L.T.; Nizio, K.D.; Wand, M.P.; Forbes, S.L. Investigating the detection limits of scent-detection dogs to residual blood odour on clothing. Forensic Chem. 2018, 9, 62–75.
  8. Frederickx, C.; Verheggen, F.J.; Haubruge, E. Biosensors in forensic sciences. Biotechnol. Agron. Soc. Environ. 2011, 15, 449–458.
  9. Hall, N.J.; Smith, D.W.; Wynne, C.D.L. Training domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) on a novel discrete trials odor-detection task. Learn. Motiv. 2013, 44, 218–228.
  10. Harper, R.J.; Furton, K.G. Biological detection of explosives. In Counterterrorist Detection Techniques of Explosives; Elsevier Science B.V.: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 395–431, ISBN 9780444522047.
  11. Rendine, M.; Fiore, C.; Bertozzi, G.; De Carlo, D.; Filetti, V.; Fortarezza, P.; Riezzo, I. Decomposing Human Blood: Canine Detection Odor Signature and Volatile Organic Compounds. J. Forensic Sci. 2018, 1–6.
  12. Sacharczuk, M.; Walczak, M.; Adamkiewicz, E.; Walasek, A.; Ensminger, J.; Presch, M.; Jezierski, T. Polymorphism of olfactory and neurotransmitters receptor genes in drug and explosives detection dogs can be associated with differences in detection performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 215, 52–60.
  13. Chen, R.; Irwin, D.M.; Zhang, Y.P. Differences in selection drive olfactory receptor genes in different directions in dogs and wolf. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2012, 29, 3475–3484
  14. Lesniak, A.; Walczak, M.; Jezierski, T.; Sacharczuk, M.; Gawkowski, M.; Jaszczak, K. Canine olfactory receptor gene polymorphism and its relation to odor detection performance by sniffer dogs. J. Hered. 2008, 99, 518–527.
  15. Quignon, P.; Rimbault, M.; Robin, S.; Galibert, F. Genetics of canine olfaction and receptor diversity. Mamm. Genome 2012, 23, 132–143.
  16. Mombaerts, P.; Wang, F.; Dulac, C.; Chao, S.K.; Nemes, A.; Mendelsohn, M.; Edmondson, J.; Axel, R. Visualizing an Olfactory Sensory Map. Cell 1965, 18, 385–387
  17. Quignon, P.; Giraud, M.; Rimbault, M.; Lavigne, P.; Tacher, S.; Morin, E.; Retout, E.; Valin, A.S.; Lindblad-Toh, K.; Nicolas, J.; et al. The dog and rat olfactory receptor repertoires. Genome Biol. 2005, 6
  18. Beebe, S.C.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. Using Scent Detection Dogs in Conservation Settings: A Review of Scientific Literature Regarding Their Selection. Front. Vet. Sci. 2016, 3, 1–13.
  19. Polgár, Z.; Kinnunen, M.; Ujváry, D.; Miklósi, A.; Gácsi, M.; Újváry, D.; Miklósi, Á.; Gácsi, M. A test of canine olfactory capacity: Comparing various dog breeds and wolves in a natural detection task. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, 1–14
  20. Tomkins, L.M.; Thomson, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. Behavioral and physiological predictors of guide dog success. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2011, 6, 178–187.
  21. Wilsson, E.; Sundgren, P.E. The use of a behaviour test for the selection of dogs for service and breeding, I: Method of testing and evaluating test results in the adult dog, demands on different kinds of service dogs, sex and breed differences. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1997, 53, 279–295.
  22. Harper, R.J.; Almirall, J.R.; Furton, K.G. Identification of dominant odor chemicals emanating from explosives for use in developing optimal training aid combinations and mimics for canine detection. Talanta 2005, 67, 313–327.
  23. Stadler, S.; Stefanuto, P.-H.; Byer, J.D.; Brokl, M.; Forbes, S.L.; Focant, J.-F. Analysis of synthetic canine training aids by comprehensive two-dimensional gas chromatography-time of flight mass spectrometry. J. Chromatogr. A 2012, 1255, 202–206.
  24. Martin, C.; Verheggen, F. Odour profile of human corpses: A review. Forensic Chem. 2018, 10, 27–36.
  25. Rosier, E.; Loix, S.; Develter, W.; Van de Voorde, W.; Tytgat, J.; Cuypers, E. Time-dependent VOC-profile of decomposed human and animal remains in laboratory environment. Forensic Sci. Int. 2016, 266, 164–169.
  26. Cernosek, T.; Eckert, K.E.; Carter, D.O.; Perrault, K.A. Volatile Organic Compound Profiling from Postmortem Microbes using Gas Chromatography–Mass Spectrometry. J. Forensic Sci. 2019.
  27. Dubois, L.M.; Stefanuto, P.H.; Perrault, K.A.; Delporte, G.; Delvenne, P.; Focant, J.F. Comprehensive Approach for Monitoring Human Tissue Degradation. Chromatographia 2019.
  28. Finley, S.J.; Benbow, M.E.; Javan, G.T. Microbial communities associated with human decomposition and their potential use as postmortem clocks. Int. J. Legal Med. 2015, 129, 623–632.
  29. Martin, C.; Vanderplanck, M.; Boullis, A.; Haubruge, E.; Verheggen, F. Impact of necrophagous insects on the emission of volatile organic compounds released during the decaying process. Entomol. Gen. 2019, 39, 19–31.
  30. Dekeirsschieter, J.; Stefanuto, P.-H.; Brasseur, C.; Haubruge, E.; Focant, J.-F. Enhanced characterization of the smell of death by comprehensive two-dimensional gas chromatography-time-of-flight mass spectrometry (GCxGC-TOFMS). PLoS ONE 2012, 7.
  31. Sinn, D.L.; Gosling, S.D.; Hilliard, S. Personality and performance in military working dogs: Reliability and predictive validity of behavioral tests. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 127, 51–65. [
  32. Hurt, A.; Smith, D. Conservation dogs. In Canine Ergonomics; Helton, W., Ed.; CRC Press: London, UK, 2009; pp. 175–194.
  33. Chesney, C.J. The microclimate of the canine coat: The effects of heating on coat and skin temperature and relative humidity. Vet. Dermatol. 1997, 8, 183–190.
  34. Bulanda, S. Ready! Training the Search and Rescue Dog, 2nd ed.; Book, K.C., Ed.; Companion House Books: Mount Joy, PA, USA, 2010.
  35. Dahlgren, D.K.; Elmore, R.D.; Smith, D.A.; Hurt, A.; Arnett, E.B.; Connelly, J.W. Use of dogs in wildlife research and management. In The Wildlife Techniques Manual, 7th ed.; The John Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2011; pp. 140–153
  36. Craven, B.A.; Paterson, E.G.; Settles, G.S. The fluid dynamics of canine olfaction: Unique nasal airflow patterns as an explanation of macrosmia. J. R. Soc. Interface 2010, 7, 933–943.
  37. Brownell, D.A.; Marsolais, M. The Brownell-Marsolais scale: A proposal for the quantitative evaluation of SAR/Disaster K9 Candidates. Adv. Rescue Technol. 2000, 57–67
  38. Lazarowski, L.; Haney, P.S.; Brock, J.; Fisher, T.; Rogers, B.; Angle, C.; Katz, J.S.; Waggoner, L.P. Investigation of the Behavioral characteristics of Dogs Purpose- Bred and Prepared to Perform Vapor Wake® Detection of Person-Borne explosives. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 20.
  39. Deldalle, S.; Gaunet, F. E ects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog-owner relationship. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2014, 9, 58–65.
  40. Doré, F.Y.; Mercier, P. Foundation of learning and cognition. Press. Universtitaire Lille
  41. Ziv, G. The e ects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2017,19, 50–60.
  42. Topoleski, J.; Schultz, C.A.;Warren,W.G. Identifying and resolving end of session cues in substance detection canine training. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 5, 1–6.
  43. Affenzeller, N.; Palme, R.; Zulch, H. Playful activity post-learning improves training performance in Labrador Retriever dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Physiol. Behav. 2017, 168, 62–73.
  44. Hoffman, E.M.; Curran, A.M.; Dulgerian, N.; Stockham, R.A.; Eckenrode, B.A. Characterization of the volatile organic compounds present in the headspace of decomposing human remains. Forensic Sci. Int. 2009, 186, 6–13.
More
Information
Subjects: Zoology; Others
Contributors MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register : , ,
View Times: 2.0K
Revisions: 3 times (View History)
Update Date: 26 Oct 2020
1000/1000
Video Production Service