Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 + 1864 word(s) 1864 2021-07-06 06:18:44 |
2 corrected the format Meta information modification 1864 2022-01-05 02:21:19 |

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Mota-Rojas, D. Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/17751 (accessed on 16 April 2024).
Mota-Rojas D. Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/17751. Accessed April 16, 2024.
Mota-Rojas, Daniel. "Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/17751 (accessed April 16, 2024).
Mota-Rojas, D. (2022, January 04). Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/17751
Mota-Rojas, Daniel. "Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding." Encyclopedia. Web. 04 January, 2022.
Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding
Edit

In buffalo, bonding is very particular due to the expression of specific behaviors, such as allo-suckling and communal rearing.

imprinting bonding animal welfare mother–young relationship buffaloes behavior welfare

1. Introduction

In most mammals, care for the young depends primarily on the mother [1]. She is the most important social contact for the newborn during the first months of life because, in addition to feeding and care, she provides the offspring with the acquisition of important information in relation to the physical and social environment [2][3]. Therefore, the bond that is generated at birth, which is dependent on a learning process known as “imprinting”, which allows the identification of the mother [4], is fundamental for the survival of the young. Imprinting is a learning mechanism which allows the rapid acquisition of a clear and stable preference for a particular type of stimulus to which the animal is exposed during a sensitive period [5][6].

A bonded mother overtly expresses maternal behavior, including suckling, and shows negative behavioral responses, such as increased vocalizations and locomotor activity, in response to separation from her young; responses that decrease after reunion [7][8]. Similarly, the bonded offspring express affiliative behavior toward the mother, and show marked distress when separated from her [8].

The period during which the bond is stablished is known as the “sensitive period”, since it is a limited stage dependent on early learning of sensory stimuli (sight, touch, smell, and hearing) exchanged naturally with the mother or the environment during the neonatal phase [2][3][4]. Any interference in this sensitive period, such as human intervention or some other external stimuli, could lead to the development of an inaccurate imprint and alterations of the bond. An extreme example has been reported in small ruminants by Kendrick et al. [9], who showed that rams that were raised by goats, or goats that were raised by sheep, preferred females of the species that raised them, rather than females of their own species as a sexual partner when adults.

Altered imprinted bonds reduce the newborn’s probability of survival and impair their behavioral development to adulthood [10]. For example, aggressiveness can increase in animals reared in isolation from social groups, which is attributed to the fact that during early stages they did not receive the necessary information or stimulation from the mother and, consequently, did not learn how to react to or interact with conspecifics in situations needing prior knowledge [11][12][13]. Therefore, the quality of the contact with the mothers can also affect the intensity of their future gregariousness as adults and the quality of their relationship with conspecifics [14][15].

2. Sensory Stimulation and Communication during the Imprinting Process

During the sensitive period, numerous sensory communication channels are deployed to ensure mutual recognition between the mother and her offspring [16][17].

Learning is a complex process that involves the acquisition, storage, and retrieval of information, all of which form memory, and consolidate learned behavior [5][18]. The only necessary condition between the association of a stimulus and a response is the close temporal relationship between them. According to Knudsen [19], learning that occurs during the sensitive period has a long-lasting influence on the development of the individual’s social and emotional behavior. During this sensitive period, most newborns have developed vision, hearing, smelling and even touching, in such a way that perhaps imprinting is triggered by the dominant sense of the species at that time [5][6].

Once this link is established, the mother becomes very selective and actively rejects any other unidentified newborn that attempts to suckle [20][21]. In cattle, maternal vision and smell are fundamental factors in establishing the cow–calf bond [22]. In dairy cows, contact for 5 min with her calf immediately after calving is sufficient for the formation of a strong and specific bond between the mother and her calf [23][24]. Although smell is referred to as the most important sensory channel in sheep [25][26] at the beginning of maternal behavior, this cannot be generalized to all species.

Tactile communication involves as main recognition factor the physical contact between individuals, which can even occur due to thermo-reception, as in human babies, sheep, puppies, and piglets. These human and nonhuman animals localize and then identify the mother’s udder/nipple to suckle based on thermo-receptors [27][28].

Licking is essential for the development and strengthening of the mother–calf bond in mammals [29]. The cow usually begins liking her calf from the head, perhaps because removal of the fetal membranes decreases the risk of suffocation. Removal of the amniotic fluids can also help to dry the newborn’s coat, thus reducing heat loss, and stimulating nipple-seeking activity [30], while also allowing the mother to learn the specific odor of her own neonate [31].

Multiparous mothers spent more time licking their calves than primiparous, which suggests a higher degree of maternal care expressed by more experienced mothers [32]. In Italian Mediterranean buffalo the dams mainly groomed the calf during the first six hours after calving, to ensure the formation of the cow–calf bond; thereafter, the behavior decreased over time [33]. Both in cattle and buffalo, cows ingest the fetal membranes during this process of conditioning and cleaning of the offspring, whereas sheep and goats normally do not express this behavior [34][30]. However, in sheep, after the sensitive period, the ewes almost never groom their lambs [35], whereas grooming is quite common in cattle even in adults [36].

Auditory imprinting relies on mother–offspring vocal communication and allows acoustic recognition. The vocalizations emitted by the newborns stimulate care and are important components in the regulation of early interactions in many species, for example, humans [37], bats [38][39], and guinea pigs [40]. Sophisticated recognition strategies have been studied in many social mammals where mother and offspring use vocalizations to find and recognize each other even after long periods of separation [41][42][43][44].

Acoustic recognition relies on the one hand on individual distinct vocalizations by the sender and on the other hand on the ability of the receiver to recognize these individually distinct vocalizations. It may also be uni- or bidirectional [41].

In farmed ruminants, the newborns vocalize a few hours after birth to attract the attention of the mother when they need to be fed or protected [45][32]. In sheep, the mother shows an intense peak of vocal activity during the first three hours after parturition [34]. After this period, the number of vocal events gradually decreases up to 24 h postpartum [46]. In cattle and sheep, auditory recognition tends to be bidirectional between the mother and her offspring, varying with age [44], breed [47], and time elapsed from bond formation [48].

Immediately after parturition, several species of ungulates, including farmed ruminants such as cattle, buffaloes, goats, and sheep are very receptive to the smell of their young [49]. Ewes need two to four hours postpartum to acquire these chemical signals, recognize their lambs and become selective, rejecting all other lambs [50][51]. In a few days, lambs learn to walk ahead of their mothers to be recognized and then take the inverse-parallel position to be definitively identified by their mothers based on the smell originated from the perianal region. In small ruminants, smell ceases to be efficient at distances greater than 0.25 m [52][53] requiring that ewes and lambs stay close to each other during the sensitive period.

During the early postpartum period, cell proliferation occurs in specific areas of the mother’s brain, as well as in the main olfactory bulb. The maternal brain is modified by remodeling neural circuits, especially olfactory structures. This process is called adult neurogenesis, a type of brain plasticity that could constitute an adaptive response to motherhood [26] and could facilitate the development of an olfactory memory. Therefore, it seems that the ability of the olfactory system to generate new interneurons plays an important role in the acceptance and recognition of the offspring during the sensitive period.

In buffalo, the information on visual communication and cow–calf recognition is nil, even though visual cues are commonly used by domestic animals including ruminants, horses, and dogs. Newborns recognize and follow the object or movable entity that provides emotional support, nutrition, and protection. Keller et al. [21] reported that in sheep, the ability to efficiently to recognize their lambs by sight at a distance is higher in multiparous than primiparous ewes.

In summary, it can be hypothesized that, once the maternal–filial bond has been formed, visual and auditory signals are used by mothers and young to orientate and localize the partner as well as for preliminary recognition at a distance, whereas smell is used for the definitive identification, which is performed when the pair is in proximity.

3. Neurotransmitters Involved in Imprinting

Imprinting involves communication within the nervous system through neurotransmitters that induce morphological alterations (neuronal plasticity) creating new connections as the offspring learns [54]. There are specific neurotransmitters that are essentials in the development of the mother–young bond.

During parturition, estrogens and oxytocin are released [55]. However, after a few hours, the concentration of these hormones decreases. If recognition did not occur during this period, the bond may not be properly established [56].

Estrogens are mainly female steroid sex hormones, produced by the ovaries, the placenta during pregnancy and, to a lesser extent, by the adrenal glands [57]. The possibility of inducing maternal behavior in non-pregnant ewes by injections of ovarian steroids has been investigated by Le Neindre et al. [58], who found clear evidence of the establishment of selective maternal behavior shown by the treated ewes. Other researchers have found that intracerebroventricular injections of oxytocin also induce maternal responses in estrogen-treated females.

Oxytocin is a hormone produced by the supraoptic and paraventricular nuclei of the hypothalamus that is released into the circulation through the neurohypophysis [57]. The role of endogenous oxytocin mediating the onset of maternal behavior has been demonstrated in numerous nonhuman species [59]. Perinatal manipulation of the oxytocin system provides strong evidence for subsequent dysfunctional maternal behaviors [60].

In nonhuman primates, optimal maternal behavior can be altered with an injection of synthetic oxytocin or an antagonist [61]. In the postpartum period, oxytocin can negatively affect anxiety and depression [62]. One of the changes that are believed to be necessary for normal maternal behavior is the decrease in anxiety during the postpartum period [63] allowing the mother to accept more easily an offspring [64].

In the central nervous system, neurons that secrete oxytocin send projections to various sites, including the amygdala, hippocampus, nucleus accumbens, and ventral tegmental area. Oxytocin is released in response to physical and psychological stressors and to various positive or satisfying social stimuli [55]. In both human and animal studies, biological systems that contribute to maternal behavior have been identified, focusing on the oxytokinergic and dopaminergic systems, as oxytocin activates dopamine pathways in response to social cues [65].

Although prolactin is best known for its role in milk production, it also plays an important role in maternal care and parental behavior in birds and mammals [66]. During pregnancy, prolactin concentration increases, stimulating neurogenesis in the subventricular zone of the lateral ventricle of the brain. New neurons produced in the forebrain during pregnancy and lactation migrate to the olfactory bulb where they likely participate in processing olfactory cues received by the new mother as she adapts to the needs and challenges of raising young [67]. Therefore, low prolactin levels during early gestation and the consequent suppression of neurogenesis in the brain are associated with increased postpartum anxiety [68].

Figure 1 illustrates the different neurotransmitters involved during the imprinting process (PVN: paraventricular nucleus).

Figure 1. Neurotransmitters involved during the imprinting process (PVN: paraventricular nucleus).

References

  1. Zilkha, N.; Sofer, Y.; Beny, Y.; Kimchi, T. From classic ethology to modern neuroethology: Overcoming the three biases in social behavior research. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2016, 38, 96–108.
  2. Mandujano-Camacho, H. Ecología y sociobiología de la impronta: Perspectivas para su estudio en los Crocodylia. Ciencia y Mar 2010, 14, 49–54.
  3. Mora-Medina, P.; Orihuela, A.; Arch-Tirado, E.; Vázquez, C.; Mota-Rojas, D. Metabolic changes during brief periods of ewe–lamb separation at different ages. Anim. Prod. Sci. 2018, 58, 1297–1306.
  4. Yamaguchi, S.; Aoki, N.; Kitajima, T.; Iikubo, E.; Katagiri, S.; Matsushima, T.; Homma, K.J. Thyroid hormone determines the start of the sensitive period of imprinting and primes later learning. Nat. Commun. 2012, 3, 1081.
  5. Fraser, A.F. Comportamiento de los Animales de Granja; Acribia: Zaragoza, España, 1980; p. 292.
  6. Maier, R. La evolución del aprendizaje. In Comportamiento Animal. Un Enfoque Evolutivo y Ecológico; Mc Graw Hill: Madrid, España, 2001; p. 541.
  7. Solano, J.; Orihuela, A.; Galina, C.S.; Aguirre, V. A note on behavioral responses to brief cow-calf separation and reunion in cattle (Bos indicus). J. Vet. Behav. 2007, 2, 10–14.
  8. Sirovnik, J.; Varth, K.; decOliveira, D.; Ferneborg, S.; Haskell, M.J.; Hillmann, E.; Jensen, M.B.; Mejdell, C.M.; Napolitano, F.; Vaarst, M.; et al. Methodological terminology and definitions for research and discussion of cow-calf contact systems. J. Dairy Res. 2020, 87, S1.
  9. Kendrick, K.M.; Hinton, M.R.; Atkins, K. Mothers determine sexual preferences. Nature 1998, 395, 229–230.
  10. Galef, B.G.; Laland, K.N. Social learning in animals: Empirical studies and theoretical models. AIBS Bull. 2005, 55, 489–499.
  11. Price, E.O.; Wallach, S.J.R. Physical isolation of hand-reared Hereford bulls increases their aggressiveness toward humans. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1999, 27, 263–267.
  12. Laland, K.N. On the evolutionary consequences of sexual imprinting. Evolution 1994, 48, 477–489.
  13. Langmore, N.E. Functions of duet and solo songs of female birds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1998, 13, 136–140.
  14. Damián, J.P.; Hötzel, M.J.; Banchero, G.; Ungerfeld, R. Growing without a mother during rearing affects the response to stressors in rams. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 209, 36–40.
  15. Damián, J.P.; Beracochea, F.; Machado, S.; Hötzel, M.J.; Banchero, G.; Ungerfeld, R. Growing without a mother results in poorer sexual behaviour in adult rams. Animal 2018, 12, 98–105.
  16. Lezama-García, K.; Mariti, C.; Mota-Rojas, D.; Martínez-Burnes, J.; Barrios-García, H.; Gazzano, A. Maternal behaviour in domestic dogs. Intl. J. Vet. Sci. Med. 2019, 7, 20–30.
  17. Wierucka, K.; Pitcher, B.J.; Harcourt, R.; Charrier, I. Multimodal mother–offspring recognition: The relative importance of sensory cues in a colonial mammal. Anim. Behav. 2018, 146, 135–142.
  18. Horn, G. Neural mechanisms of learning: An analysis of imprinting in the domestic chick. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Series B 1981, 213, 101–137.
  19. Knudsen, E.I. Sensitive Periods in the Development of the Brain and Behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 1412–1425.
  20. Poindron, P. Mechanisms of activation of maternal behaviour in mammals. Reprod. Nutr. Dev. 2005, 45, 341–351.
  21. Keller, M.; Meurisse, M.; Poindron, P.; Nowak, R.; Ferreira, G.; Shayit, M.; Levy, F. Maternal experience influences the establishment of visual/auditory, but not olfactory recognition of the newborn lamb by ewes at parturition. Dev. Psychobiol. 2003, 43, 167–176.
  22. Griffith, M.K.; Williams, G.L. Contribution of maternal vision and olfaction to suckling-mediated inhibition of LH secretion, the expression of maternal selectivity, and lactation in beef cows. Biol. Reprod. 1996, 54, 761–768.
  23. Hudson, S.J.; Mullord, M.M. Investigations of maternal bonding in dairy cattle. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 1977, 3, 271–276.
  24. Johnsen, J.F.; Zipp, K.A.; Kälber, T.; de Passillé, A.M.; Knierim, U.; Barth, K.; Mejdell, C.M. Is rearing calves with the dam a feasible option for dairy farms? Current and future research. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 181, 1–11.
  25. Lévy, F.; Keller, M.; Poindron, P. Olfactory regulation of maternal behavior in mammals. Hom. Behav. 2004, 46, 284–302.
  26. Corona, R.; Meurisse, M.; Cornilleau, F.; Moussu, C.; Keller, M.; Lévy, F. Disruption of adult olfactory neurogenesis induces deficits in maternal behavior in sheep. Behav. Brain Res. 2018, 347, 124–131.
  27. Arenas, A. Aprendizaje olfativo temprano en la abeja (Apis mellifera) y su rol en la toma de Decisiones Relacionadas con la obtención de recursos. Ph.D. Thesis, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina, 2009.
  28. Rosillon-Warnier, A.; Paquay, R. Development and consequences of teat-order in piglets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1984, 13, 47–58.
  29. Matamala, F.; Strappini, A.; Sepúlveda-Varas, P. Dairy cow behaviour around calving: Its relationship with management practices and environmental conditions. Austral J. Vet. Sci. 2021, 53, 9–22.
  30. Nowak, R.; Porter, R.H.; Levy, F.; Orgeur, P.; Schaal, B. Role of mother-young interactions in the survival of offspring in domestic mammals. Rev. Reprod. 2000, 5, 153–163.
  31. Bordi, F.; LeDoux, J.E. Response properties of single unit I areas of rat auditory thalamus that project to the amygdala. Exp. Brain Res. 1994, 98, 261–274.
  32. Yadav, A.K.; Pramanik, P.S.; Kashyap, S.S. Dam-calf interactions in Murrah buffaloes up to six hours post-parturition. Indian J. Anim. Prod. Manag. 2009, 25, 78–80.
  33. Lanzoni, L.; Chincarini, M.; Giammarco, M.; Fusaro, I.; Gloria, A.; Contri, A.; Ferri, N.; Vignola, G. Materal and neonatal behaviour in Italian Mediterranean Buffaloes. Animals 2021, 11, 1584.
  34. Mora-Medina, P.; Orihuela, A.; Arch-Tirado, E.; Roldan-Santiago, P.; Terrazas, A.; Mota-Rojas, D. Sensory factors involved in mother-young bonding in sheep: A review. Vet. Med-Czech. 2016, 61, 595e611.
  35. Nowak, R.; Boivin, X. Filial attachment in sheep: Similarities and differences between ewe-lamb and human-lamb relationships. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 164, 12–28.
  36. Sato, S.; Sako, S.; Maeda, A. Social licking patterns in cattle (Bos taurus): Influence of environmental and social factors. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1991, 32, 3–12.
  37. Mills, M.; Melhuish, E. Recognition of mother’s voice in early infancy. Nature 1974, 252, 123–124.
  38. Balcombe, P. Vocal recognition of pups by mother Mexican free-tailed bats, Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana. Anim. Behav. 1990, 39, 60–66.
  39. Knörnschild, M.; Von Helversen, O. Nonmutual vocal mother–pup recognition in the greater sac-winged bat. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 1001–1009.
  40. Kober, M.; Trillmich, F.; Naguib, M. Vocal mother-offspring communication in guinea pigs: Females adjust maternal responsiveness to litter size. Front. Zool. 2008, 5, 13.
  41. Torriani, M.V.G.; Vannoni, E.; McElliogot, A.G. Mother-Young recognition in an Ungulate Hider Species: An unidirectional process. Am. Naturalist 2006, 168, 412–420.
  42. Charrier, I.; Burlet, A.; Aubin, T. Social vocal communication in captive Pacific walruses Odobenus rosmarus divergens. Mamm. Biol. 2010, 76, 622–627.
  43. Briefer, E.; McElligott, A.G. Mutual mother-offspring vocal recognition in an ungulate hiders species (Capra hircus). Anim. Cong. 2011, 14, 585–598.
  44. De la Torre, M.P.; Briefer, E.F.; Ochocki, B.M.; McElligott, A.G.; Reader, T. Mother–offspring recognition via contact calls in cattle, Bos taurus. Anim. Behav. 2016, 114, 147–154.
  45. Dubey, P.; Singh, R.R.; Choudhary, S.S.; Verma, K.K.; Kumar, A.; Gamit, P.M.; Dubey, S.; Prajapati, K. Post parturient neonatal behaviour and their relationship with maternal behaviour score, parity and sex in Surti buffaloes. J. Appl. Anim. Res. 2018, 46, 360–364.
  46. Sèbe, F.; Nowak, R.; Poindron, P.; Aubin, T. Establishment of vocal communication and discrimination between ewes and their lamb in the first two days after parturition. Dev. Psychobiol. 2007, 49, 375–386.
  47. Pickup, H.; Dwyer, C. Breed differences in the expression of maternal care at parturition persist throughout the lactation period in sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 132, 33–41.
  48. Sèbe, F.; Duboscq, J.; Aubin, T.; Ligout, S.; Poindron, P. Early vocal recognition of mother by lambs: Contribution of low- and high- frequency vocalizations. Anim. Behav. 2010, 79, 1055–1066.
  49. Sánchez-Andrade, G.; James, B.M.; Kendrick, K.M. Neural encoding of olfactory recognition memory. J. Reprod. Dev. 2005, 51, 547–558.
  50. Poindron, P.; Levy, F.; Keller, M. Maternal responsiveness and maternal selectivity in domestic sheep and goat: The two facets of maternal attachments. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 49, 54–70.
  51. Hernández, H.; Terrazas, A.; Poindron, P.; Ramírez-Vera, S.; Flores, J.A.; Delgadillo, J.A.; Vielma, J.; Duarte, G.; Fernández, I.G.; Fitz-Rodríguez, G.; et al. Sensorial and physiological control of maternal behavior in small ruminants: Sheep and goats. Trop. Subtrop. Agroecosys. 2012, 15, s91–s102.
  52. Alexander, G. Odour, and the recognition of lambs by Merino ewes. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 1978, 4, 153–158.
  53. Poindron, P.; Gilling, G.; Hernandez, H.; Serafin, N.; Terrazas, A. Early recognition of newborn goat kids by their mother: I. Nonolfactory discrimination. Dev. Psychobiol. 2003, 43, 82–89.
  54. Castro-Sierra, E.; Chico, P.L.F.; Gordillo, D.L.F.; Portugal, R.A. Neurotransmisores del sistema límbico. Hipocampo, GABA y memoria. Primera parte. Salud Mental. 2007, 30, 7–15.
  55. Love, T.M. Oxytocin, motivation and the role of Dopamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2014, 119, 49–60.
  56. Mora-Medina, P.; Napolitano, F.; Mota-Rojas, D.; Berdugo-Gutiérrez, J.; Ruiz-Buitrago, J.; Guerrero-Legarreta, I. Imprinting, Sucking and Allosucking Behaviors in Buffalo Calves. J. Buffalo Sci. 2018, 7, 49–57.
  57. Acevedo-Rodríguez, A.; Mani, S.K.; Handa, R.J. Oxytocin and estrogen receptor β in the brain: An overview. Front. Endocrinol. 2015.
  58. Le Neindre, P.; Poindron, P.; Delouis, C. Hormonal induction of maternal behavior in non-pregnant ewes. Physiol. Behav. 1979, 22, 731–734.
  59. Pedersen, C.A.; Caldwell, J.D.; Walker, C.; Ayers, G.; Mason, G.A. Oxytocin activates the postpartum onset of rat maternal behavior in the ventral tegmental and medial preoptic areas. Behav. Neurosci. 1994, 108, 1163–1171.
  60. Boccia, M.L.; Goursaud, A.P.S.; Bachevalier, J.; Anderson, K.D.; Pedersen, C.A. Peripherally administered non-peptide oxytocin antagonist, L368,899®, accumulates in limbic brain areas: A new pharmacological tool for the study of social motivation in non-human primates. Horm. Behav. 2007, 52, 344–351.
  61. Keverne, E.B.; Kendrick, K.M. Oxytocin facilitation of maternal behavior in sheep. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1992, 652, 83–101.
  62. Kim, S.; Soken, T.A.; Cromer, S.J.; Martínez, S.R.; Hardy, L.R.; Strathearn, L. Oxytocin and postpartum depression: Delivering on what´s known and what´s not. Brain Res. 2014, 11, 219–232.
  63. Rich, M.E.; de Cárdenas, E.J.; Lee, H.J.; Caldwell, H.K. Impairments in the Initiation of Maternal Behavior in Oxytocin Receptor Knockout Mice. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e98839.
  64. Caldwell, H.K. Neurobiology of sociability. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 2012, 739, 187–205.
  65. Strathearn, L. Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of attachment. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2011, 23, 1054–1065.
  66. Chaiseha, Y.; Ngernsoungner, P.; Sartsoongnoen, N.; Prakobsaeng, N.; El Halawani, M.E. Presence of prolactin mRNA in extra-pituitary brain areas in the domestic turkey. Acta Histochem. 2012, 114, 116–121.
  67. Bridges, R.S.; Gratta, D.R. Prolactin-induced neurogenesis in the maternal brain. Trends Endocrin. Mets. 2003, 14, 199–201.
  68. Larsen, C.M.; Grattan, D.R. Prolactin, neurogenesis, and maternal behaviors. Brain Behav. Immun. 2012, 26, 201–209.
More
Information
Subjects: Zoology
Contributor MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register :
View Times: 756
Revisions: 2 times (View History)
Update Date: 19 Apr 2022
1000/1000