Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 -- 5606 2024-03-11 12:49:24 |
2 layout Meta information modification 5606 2024-03-12 02:05:33 |

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Siligato, E.; Iuele, G.; Barbera, M.; Bruno, F.; Tordonato, G.; Mautone, A.; Rizzo, A. Freezing Effect and Bystander Effect. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/56112 (accessed on 29 April 2024).
Siligato E, Iuele G, Barbera M, Bruno F, Tordonato G, Mautone A, et al. Freezing Effect and Bystander Effect. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/56112. Accessed April 29, 2024.
Siligato, Elena, Giada Iuele, Martina Barbera, Francesca Bruno, Guendalina Tordonato, Aurora Mautone, Amelia Rizzo. "Freezing Effect and Bystander Effect" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/56112 (accessed April 29, 2024).
Siligato, E., Iuele, G., Barbera, M., Bruno, F., Tordonato, G., Mautone, A., & Rizzo, A. (2024, March 11). Freezing Effect and Bystander Effect. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/56112
Siligato, Elena, et al. "Freezing Effect and Bystander Effect." Encyclopedia. Web. 11 March, 2024.
Freezing Effect and Bystander Effect
Edit

As a passive and defensive response to a stressful event, freezing is characterized by a reduction in body movements, bradycardia (a decrease in heart rate), and an increase in muscle tone. The phenomenon of freezing is commonly linked with fear and is believed to enhance processes related to perception and attention, which help in identifying signals that dictate suitable actions. The phenomenon of bystander inaction, commonly referred to as the bystander effect or bystander apathy, is a psychological and social occurrence where an individual observing an emergency situation fails to assist the person in distress.

freezing effect bystander effect trauma

1. Introduction

The bystander effect and freezing phenomenon share similarities in that they result in inaction and an inability to intervene. However, they also have distinct differences. Although both are significant in social psychology, there has been a lack of systematic comparison between these two concepts.

2. The Freezing Effect

Gray’s hypothesis posits a neurobehavioral mechanism that governs human defensive behaviors in response to both innate and learned threat stimuli, with these responses phenomenologically correlating with the subjective experience of fear [1]. His theory aimed to deepen the relationship between the neurobiological systems regulating approach and avoidance behaviors. As known, Cannon [2] wrote about these two strategies of approaching threatening events/stimuli in emergency situations, which were later elaborated by Gray in the initial version of his theory as the FFS (fight–flight system), alongside two other response systems to threatening stimuli: the BAS (behavioral activation system) and the BIS (behavioral inhibition system). The FFS was revised in 2000 by Gray and MacNaughton [3], becoming the FFFS (fight–flight–freeze system), thus incorporating the behavioral response of immobilization (freezing) in relation to aversive stimuli. This third option is characterized by an automatic alert action associated with fear, with postural stiffening and a decrease in cardiovascular and somatomotor activity [1][3]. Evolutionarily, indeed, prey that remains immobilized during a threatening situation has much less of a chance of being captured. This mechanism constitutes a key component of the reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST), which pertains to personality. The fight–flight–freeze system, operational upon the perception of threat, orchestrates adaptive responses to aversive stimuli, with fear being the subjective psychological state accompanying these responses. This system is delineated by a repertoire of defensive behaviors, which encompasses immobilization (freezing), rapid evasion (flight), and aggressive confrontation (fight) [4].
As a passive and defensive response to a stressful event, freezing is characterized by a reduction in body movements, bradycardia (a decrease in heart rate), and an increase in muscle tone [5]. The phenomenon of freezing is commonly linked with fear and is believed to enhance processes related to perception and attention, which help in identifying signals that dictate suitable actions [6][7]. Unlike extensive studies on freezing in animals and investigations into other human stress responses like fight or flight, research focusing on human freezing is quite limited. The concept of fight or flight as a human reaction to stress was established in the 1920s, but the idea of freezing as a third response only gained attention about half a century later and has not been thoroughly explored. In the animal kingdom, freezing in response to threats can be seen as an effective tactic, akin to feigning death in dangerous situations. In humans, however, freezing often translates to a paralysis of sorts, marked by an inability to communicate, respond, or engage in any actions for self-defense or preservation [8][9][10][11][12] (see Figure 1).
Figure 1. Features of freezing effect map.

2.1. Fight or Flight Response and Freezing Mechanism

Fight, flight, and freeze are three distinct physiological reactions that the body initiates in response to perceived danger or threat. Each of these responses is characterized by unique features. The fight response is a proactive defense mechanism activated when an individual assesses that they have sufficient resources to confront a threat. This response involves the activation of the autonomic nervous system, leading to increased muscle activity and heart rate, priming the body for potential confrontation [11].
Flight, on the other hand, is another proactive defense response. It is triggered when an individual believes they can escape the threat and reach safety. This response also activates the autonomic nervous system, resulting in increased heart rate and lung ventilation, thus providing more oxygen to the muscles to facilitate a quicker escape [6][7].
Freeze is a passive defense response that occurs when an individual perceives no viable option for either fight or flight. This response involves the autonomic nervous system inducing a state of immobility, with reduced muscle activity, heart rate, and blood pressure. The objective is to remain inconspicuous and hope the threat passes without attack [7][8].
While fight and flight are linked to the activation of the sympathetic nervous system, which prepares the body for these active responses, they involve different neural mechanisms. The fight response is connected to the activation of the amygdala and limbic system, responsible for threat assessment and emotions like anger and aggression. It also involves the motor system and the prefrontal cortex, which play roles in movement planning and execution. The flight response, in contrast, involves the amygdala and other limbic regions like the hippocampus, crucial for memory and spatial navigation. This response also activates leg muscles and the motor area of the cerebral cortex, responsible for generating and executing movements [9][10]. In essence, while fight and flight both engage the sympathetic nervous system and certain brain regions, they represent distinct behavioral strategies in response to threats.

2.2. The Neural Basis of the Freezing Effect

Upon perceiving a threat, the brain activates a range of neural pathways to cope with the stressor; the autonomic nervous system (ANS) plays a key role in this process. During the freeze response, both branches of the ANS, the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems, are engaged [12]. It is important to recognize that freezing’s physiological characteristics are a blend of both sympathetic and parasympathetic influences, with the dominance of one system fluctuating.
The sympathetic nervous system’s response is characterized by heightened alertness and physical symptoms that support freezing: increased heart rate and cardiac output, elevated blood pressure, reduced digestive activity, enhanced respiration facilitating blood flow to active tissues, muscle tension, and pain suppression [10].
Conversely, the activation of the parasympathetic nervous system during freezing often leads to slowed heart rate [13]. This dominance is marked by either a pronounced slowing of heart rate or a less significant heart rate acceleration [14].
Changes in respiratory rates and vocalizations are also linked to freezing. In rats, rapid breathing precedes ultrasonic vocalizations, which then slow down due to the longer expiration times required for these sounds. Decreased vocalizations in rats have been associated with fear in response to immediate threats, while increased vocalizations are noted in anxiety about potential threats [15].
Stress triggers the swift activation of the sympatho-adrenomedullary (SAM) system, releasing adrenaline and noradrenaline. The sympathetic nervous system’s responses, including pupil dilation, accelerated heart rate, and increased muscle tone, are primarily driven by these neurotransmitters. Noradrenergic projections from the locus coeruleus to the dorsolateral periaqueductal gray (dlPAG) [16], primarily influenced by acetylcholine [17], facilitate the shift from freezing to active fear responses [18].
The hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis activation leads to the release of hormones such as corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), and cortisol (or cortisone in humans). CRH plays a crucial role in coordinating behavioral and metabolic threat responses across various brain regions, including the amygdala, and is vital for the expression of freezing in both primates and rodents [19].
Elevated levels of cortisol, whether baseline or stress-induced, have been linked to increased freezing in primates and rodents [20]. Glucocorticoids significantly contribute to developing defensive freezing. In neonatal rats, removing adrenal glands reduces freezing, which can be reversed with cortisol treatment [21]. Maternal care and postnatal adjustments in rats also decrease later cortisol responses to stress and are related to lower freezing reactions [22]. Interestingly, a correlation has been observed in human infants between endogenous cortisol levels, freezing, and fear bradycardia, which is not seen in more sympathetically driven fear behaviors [23].
Other hormones and peptides, such as progesterone, testosterone, estrogen, oxytocin, and vasopressin, also influence freezing [24]. For example, oxytocin affects the shift from freezing to active defense by interacting with cholinergic transmission in the amygdala’s lateral nucleus and the ACC, and by inhibiting vasopressin neurons in the amygdala’s medial central nucleus projecting toward the ventral lateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG) [25].
These hormones and peptides also interact with neurotransmitter systems involved in freezing, including gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), dopamine, and serotonin. GABA generally suppresses defensive behavior in areas like the amygdala, hypothalamus, and PAG, an effect opposed by excitatory amino acids [25]. Serotonin release in the dlPAG and ventrolateral rostral medulla inhibits active fight or flight behaviors [26].

2.3. Psychological Consequences of Freezing Effect: From Immediately to the Long-Term

The freezing response is associated with various anxiety and stress-related disorders, with long-term effects on cortisol levels as well [20][27]. Some studies [27][28] refer to the existence of cognitive mechanisms underlying freezing responses common in multiple forms of anxiety, which are thus considered vulnerability factors. In particular, the construct of “Looming Cognitive Style” (LCS) has been mentioned, which consists of a personal tendency to respond with freezing to threatening situations and is characterized by a predisposition to anxiety and perceiving a threat as more dangerous than it actually is. In light of this theory, individuals exhibiting “High Physical Looming” (HPL) demonstrate maladaptive behavior with a “dysfunctional freezing” response to a stimulus, regardless of the level of threat.

3. The Bystander Effect

3.1. Definition and Brief Analysis of the Phenomenon

The phenomenon of bystander inaction, commonly referred to as the bystander effect or bystander apathy, is a psychological and social occurrence where an individual observing an emergency situation fails to assist the person in distress [29]. This phenomenon is closely associated with the number of observers present; as the number of bystanders increases, the likelihood of any one individual providing help decreases. Factors contributing to the bystander effect include ambiguity, group cohesion, and a diffusion of responsibility. Darley and Latané conducted several experiments that have become keystones in social psychology. Typically, these experiments involved participants being placed either alone or amongst a group of other participants or confederates. During these sessions, an emergency situation would be simulated, and the researchers would observe and record the time taken by the participants to respond, if at all. These studies consistently demonstrated that the presence of others significantly deterred individual assistance, often by a substantial margin.

3.2. The Background: Kitty Genovese’s Case

As is well known, psychologists John Darley and Bibb Latané [29] were the pioneers in empirically demonstrating how the presence of other people influences individual reactions in emergency situations, in a controlled laboratory environment. Their research was motivated by the 1964 case of Kitty Genovese, a New York woman who was tragically stabbed to death near her home in Queens. On the night of 13 March 1964, in New York City, at around 3:00, this 29-year-old woman was fatally stabbed near her home. She had parked her car approximately 30 m from her residence and was viciously attacked on her way home. The assailant initially fled in his car when he noticed neighbors peering out their windows, drawn in by Genovese’s cries for help. However, he later returned and found Genovese in the entryway of a building, where he ultimately murdered her. The unique aspect of this crime was the number of witnesses; 38 people observed the incident from their homes, alerted by Genovese’s screams. Many of these witnesses believed that their individual intervention was unnecessary, assuming that “someone else must have seen more and already called the police,” a phenomenon later termed the “diffusion of responsibility” [30].

3.3. From the Real World to the Laboratory

All their experiments aimed to understand the group dynamics during such incidents. In their initial experiment, college students were invited for what was presented as a casual conversation about university life and related concerns. Each participant was isolated in a room, equipped with headphones and a microphone, and communicated with others via an intercom system, a setup intended to maintain anonymity [30].
Participants were categorized into three groups based on their perceived social setting: the first group believed they were in a one-on-one conversation, the second group thought they were communicating with two others, and the third group assumed they were part of a five-person discussion. During the conversation, a participant, simulated via the intercom, appeared to suffer a convulsion and requested help. This setup allowed Latané and Darley to assess behavioral differences based on the perceived number of witnesses. Results showed that 85% of participants in the one-on-one scenario sought help, while this figure decreased to 64% when participants believed two others were present, and further dropped to 31% in the presence of four bystanders.
In a subsequent experiment, the researchers recruited students for a purported “questionnaire task”. Participants were divided into two scenarios: some completed the questionnaire alone, while others did so in a room with several non-reactive collaborators (confederates). Shortly after beginning the task, black smoke began to seep out of the air conditioner, gradually filling the room. In the group with indifferent confederates, only 10% of participants left to report the smoke, taking twice as long as those who were alone, of whom 75% quickly sought help. This striking outcome corroborated the findings from the first experiment, reinforcing the idea that the likelihood of individual intervention diminishes as the number of bystanders increases.

3.4. Victim and Bystander: Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder

Psychological and physiological responses can arise from a variety of traumatic stressors, even those often deemed ordinary, affecting individuals who were previously in good mental health [31][32][33][34][35][36]. As is well known, repetitive abuse may affect bystanders and victims in similarly serious ways with the same levels of distress over time [33]. In fact, according to Lazarus and Folkman, psychological stress occurs when individuals perceive their interactions with their environment as potentially detrimental to their well-being. Specifically, negative evaluations of an experience, such as witnessing an act of bullying, can trigger negative emotions that lead to bystander reactions [34].
In its revised Criterion A, the DSM-5 presents a more stringent definition of trauma. This updated criterion specifies that trauma must be experienced either through direct personal involvement in the traumatic event or by being an immediate witness to such an event. It also extends to situations where individuals are informed about traumatic events that have affected close family members or friends, specifically those involving violence or accidental death. Additionally, the criterion covers repeated or intense exposure to the graphic details of severe trauma, but this exposure must be in person, thereby excluding incidents only experienced through electronic media, except in professional contexts [37]. This shift emphasizes the individual’s subjective experience of trauma, suggesting that trauma is largely determined by personal perception [38]. Contemporary research is now delving into typical stressful experiences and the conditions that may lead to varying levels of trauma, acknowledging its detrimental effect on development [39].
A critical element influencing the development and severity of traumatic symptoms is repeated exposure [40][41][42][43], which greatly increases the likelihood of significant disruptions in trust and functioning. These disruptions often necessitate the re-evaluation of fundamental beliefs about oneself, others, and the world [44][45]. Such cumulative impacts can affect every aspect of human functioning, including physical and emotional health, personality, social relationships, cognitive abilities, spirituality, and behavior [46][47][48].
Children and adolescents exhibit a heightened vulnerability to traumatic experiences, which can exert a significant and enduring impact on their developmental trajectory [49][50][51]. Research suggests that exposure to stressors, which may be perceived as normal or minor, such as bullying, harassment, or teasing, can precipitate long-term detrimental consequences [52][53][54][55][56]. Chronic exposure in this demographic is associated with amplified distress and an increased manifestation of symptoms [57][58][59][60][61]. These experiences hold potential implications for behavioral patterns and neurobiological development [57]. Despite these risks, adults often downplay these experiences, viewing them as minor forms of trauma, even considering them part of normal development [58]. This attitude results in limited support for direct victims and none for bystanders, leading children and adolescents to feel that adults are indifferent, unaware, or even approving of such abuse [59][60].
In the 1990s, the connection between such forms of abuse and youth suicides [61] and school violence [62] gained increased recognition. Childhood experiences of low-level abuse are no longer seen as trivial or harmless but are acknowledged for their potential harm to both the abused and those around them. The impact extends beyond direct victims to include bystanders, who can experience traumatic responses similar to those of the victims, blurring the line between victim and observer [63]. Studies have noted similarities in symptoms between bystanders and victims, including physiological arousal, reduced empathy [64], desensitization to negative behaviors in schools [65][66][67], and overall risky and negative behaviors [53][55]. Common feelings of isolation and inefficacy [53] are also observed. There is a growing argument for considering witnesses as co-victims or indirect victims [66], highlighting the need to recognize and support those indirectly affected by traumatic events (see Figure 2).
Figure 2. Features of bystander effect map.

3.5. Psychological Consequences in Bystander Effect: Now and in the Future

According to a study by Itzkovich et al., bystanders in cases of bullying are indirect victims, “by proxy”, a position that affects their well-being and psychological health. In fact, a correlation has been demonstrated between witnessing an act of bullying and suicidal ideation, depressive symptoms, anxiety, and stress, as well as repression of empathy among bystanders and increased feelings of guilt [65]. The theoretical model employed by the authors is the COR (conservation of resources) theory, through which it is possible to observe bystanders’ reactions to bullying in light of their individual coping resources, which help reduce exposure to stressors. Knauf et al. [66] focused on various determinants such as moral disengagement, empathy, self-efficacy, and feelings of responsibility as underlying cognitive–affective processes preceding bystanders’ reactions. Bandura [67] argued about moral disengagement as a mediator between moral reasoning and action, and as a self-regulation process to decrease tension associated with adopting behaviors contrary to morality/ethics. Byers [68] suggests that bystanders tend to resort to moral disengagement due to feelings of anxiety and frustration as coping mechanisms. Self-efficacy is supposed to influence bystander behavior according to various research; in particular, people with a lack of confidence exhibit this behavior more frequently instead of protective/proactive behaviors [69]. On the other hand, a high level of social support and of personal self-efficacy promotes defender behavior instead of bystander behavior.

4. A Comparison between the Two Phenomena

As seen in the previous paragraphs, the bystander effect and freezing phenomenon have various points of contact, such as behavior, but also some necessary distinctions, for example, in the neural regions involved or in terms of affective reactions. From a theoretical perspective, despite being two mechanisms of great interest in the field of social psychology, there are no studies that have attempted to systematically compare these two constructs. The purpose of this research was to compare these two mechanisms, starting from the definition, with respect to emotional, behavioral, neurobiological reactions, and psychological outcomes. The following table (Table 1) represents the details of the comparison for each sub-category and highlights points of contact and differences.
Table 1. Comparison between the two Phenomena.

5. Discussion

The results of this comparison indicate that the bystander effect and freezing share numerous points of contact but also have some distinctive mechanisms.
From an emotional standpoint, the bystander effect is characterized by the fear of social judgment and emotional dissonance, while freezing is characterized by a sense of helplessness, hypersensitivity, hypervigilance, and feelings of emotional detachment [71][72][73][74].
From a behavioral perspective, both the bystander effect and freezing involve immobility as a behavioral response, but they differ regarding decision-making difficulty, cognitive dissonance, the role of the diffusion of responsibility, conformity to others’ behavior, and emotional involvement [75][76][77].
In terms of neurobiology, there are some similarities and differences between the bystander effect and freezing in the brain areas involved. Both effects show reduced activation of the prefrontal cortex, which is involved in planning, processing social information, and risk assessment. In the bystander effect, the reduced activation might be related to decreased individual motivation or attention to the situation. In freezing, on the other hand, the prefrontal cortex might be involved in evaluating the situation and regulating emotional responses [78].
Another area that shows similarities is the activation of the amygdala, involved in emotional response and the detection of emotions such as fear. However, in the bystander effect, reduced amygdala activation is observed, likely due to the attenuation of individual emotional involvement determined by the presence of other people. In contrast, in freezing, the amygdala may show increased activation in response to the perception of danger or threat [79][80].
The motor cortex is another area where similarities are observed. In both the bystander effect and freezing, reduced activation of the motor cortex is observed. This is due to the common characteristics of immobility and the inhibition of motor responses in both phenomena.
Differences emerge in other areas involved. In the case of the bystander effect, reduced activation of the anterior cingulate cortex, which is involved in regulating emotions, attention, and evaluating situations of error or conflict, is observed. This reduced activation suggests lower awareness or monitoring of emergency situations in the bystander effect. In freezing, however, the hippocampus is involved in processing and remembering information related to the situation of danger or threat [75].
Other differences are found in the involvement of the thalamus nucleus and brainstem nuclei. In freezing, the thalamus nucleus plays a role in filtering and transmitting sensory information relevant to the perception of danger, while brainstem nuclei, such as the locus coeruleus and the raphe nucleus, are activated to prepare the body to respond to the threat or danger. In the case of the bystander effect, however, the activation of the thalamus nucleus and brainstem nuclei is not specifically mentioned.
In summary, the bystander effect and freezing show similarities in the brain areas involved, such as the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and motor cortex. However, they differ in other involved regions, such as the anterior cingulate cortex, hippocampus, thalamus nucleus, and brainstem nuclei, which show specific activations for each phenomenon.
Finally, the psychological consequences of the bystander effect and freezing also have some similarities and differences. Both effects, the bystander effect and freezing, can generate a sense of guilt and remorse. Those involved may feel responsible for not taking action or providing help when necessary, leading to feelings of guilt and remorse [81]. In both cases, these feelings can stem from an awareness of their own inaction and the potential contribution to the worsening of the situation or harm to the victim. Shame and a compromised sense of self-esteem are common psychological consequences in both the bystander effect and freezing. Those involved may feel inadequate or unable to intervene, negatively impacting their self-perception. This sense of shame and compromised self-esteem can be fueled by individuals’ awareness of themselves being spectators in a dangerous situation or not reacting appropriately.
The increase in anxiety and stress is another shared consequence in both the bystander effect and freezing. Awareness of the dangerous situation or the responsibility to intervene can lead to a sense of restlessness and worry. In both cases, those involved may experience heightened anxiety and emotional stress related to the situation.
However, there are also some specific psychological consequences for each phenomenon. In the case of the bystander effect, there is a reduction in empathy toward the victim or the person in danger. The presence of other people who do not react can create a social climate where individual empathy is suppressed or minimized [71]. In freezing, on the other hand, there is a sense of helplessness or inability to act, which can lead to frustration and a loss of confidence in one’s ability to handle dangerous situations. Furthermore, in the bystander effect, there is a reduction in autonomy and self-efficacy. Those involved may feel powerless or incapable of making decisions and acting independently, creating a perception of reduced self-efficacy. In freezing, effects on personal safety can develop, with a perception of vulnerability or insecurity about one’s ability to defend oneself or face similar situations in the future.
Finally, both effects can have long-term consequences on mental health. In the case of the bystander effect, persistent guilt, shame, and remorse can contribute to the development of disorders such as anxiety, depression, or post-traumatic stress disorder. In freezing, feelings of helplessness, guilt, or shame can have similar long-term effects on mental health, contributing to the development of disorders like anxiety, depression, or post-traumatic stress disorder [82][83][84][85][86].

References

  1. Gray, J.A. Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cogn. Emot. 1990, 4, 269–288.
  2. Cannon, L.B. Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear and Rage; Branford: Boston, MA, USA, 1929.
  3. Gray, J.A.; McNaughton, N. The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System; Clarendon Press/Oxford University: Oxford, UK, 2000.
  4. Roelofs, K. Freeze for action: Neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2017, 372, 20160206.
  5. Roelofs, K.; Hagenaars, M.A.; Stins, J. Facing freeze: Social threat induces bodily freeze in humans. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 1575–1581.
  6. Hagenaars, M.A.; Roelofs, K.; Stins, J.F. Human freezing in response to affective films. Anxiety Stress Coping 2014, 27, 27–37.
  7. Hagenaars, M.A.; Stins, J.F.; Roelofs, K. Aversive life events enhance human freezing responses. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012, 141, 98.
  8. Sagliano, L.; Cappuccio, A.; Trojano, L.; Conson, M. Approaching threats elicit a freeze-like response in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 2014, 561, 35–40.
  9. Lojowska, M.; Ling, S.; Roelofs, K.; Hermans, E.J. Visuocortical changes during a freezing-like state in humans. Neuroimage 2018, 179, 313–325.
  10. McCarty, R. The fight-or-flight response: A cornerstone of stress research. In Stress: Concepts, Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016; pp. 33–37.
  11. Spierer, D.K.; Griffiths, E.; Sterland, T. Fight or Flight; The Tactical Edge: Clarksville, TN, USA, 2009.
  12. Jansen, A.S.; Van Nguyen, X.; Karpitskiy, V.; Mettenleiter, T.C.; Loewy, A.D. Central command neurons of the sympathetic nervous system: Basis of the fight-or-flight response. Science 1995, 270, 644–646.
  13. Taelman, J.; Vandeput, S.; Spaepen, A.; Van Huffel, S. Influence of mental stress on heart rate and heart rate variability. In Proceedings of the 4th European Conference of the International Federation for Medical and Biological Engineering: ECIFMBE 2008, Antwerp, Belgium, 23–27 November 2008; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009; pp. 1366–1369.
  14. Appelhans, B.M.; Luecken, L.J. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2006, 10, 229–240.
  15. Misslin, R. The defense system of fear: Behavior and neurocircuitry. Neurophysiol. Clin. /Clin. Neurophysiol. 2003, 33, 55–66.
  16. Dampney, R.A. Central mechanisms regulating coordinated cardiovascular and respiratory function during stress and arousal. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2015, 309, R429–R443.
  17. Uvnäs-Moberg, K.; Arn, I.; Magnusson, D. The psychobiology of emotion: The role of the oxytocinergic system. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2005, 12, 59–65.
  18. Bracha, H.S. Freeze, flight, fight, fright, faint: Adaptationist perspectives on the acute stress response spectrum. CNS Spectr. 2004, 9, 679–685.
  19. Kalin, N.H.; Shelton, S.E.; Fox, A.S.; Oakes, T.R.; Davidson, R.J. Brain regions associated with the expression and contextual regulation of anxiety in primates. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 58, 796–804.
  20. Kalin, N.H.; Fox, A.S.; Kovner, R.; Riedel, M.K.; Fekete, E.M.; Roseboom, P.H.; Tromp, D.P.; Grabow, B.P.; Olsen, M.E.; Brodsky, E.K.; et al. Overexpressing corticotropin-releasing factor in the primate amygdala increases anxious temperament and alters its neural circuit. Biol. Psychiatry 2016, 80, 345–355.
  21. Hashemi, M.M.; Zhang, W.; Kaldewaij, R.; Koch, S.B.; Smit, A.; Figner, B.; Jonker, R.; Klumpers, F.; Roelofs, K. Human defensive freezing: Associations with hair cortisol and trait anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2021, 133, 105417.
  22. Roelofs, K.; Elzinga, B.M.; Rotteveel, M. The effects of stress-induced cortisol responses on approach–avoidance behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2005, 30, 665–677.
  23. Niermann, H.C.; Figner, B.; Tyborowska, A.; van Peer, J.M.; Cillessen, A.H.; Roelofs, K. Defensive freezing links Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal-axis activity and internalizing symptoms in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017, 82, 83–90.
  24. Dzirbikova, Z.; Talarovičová, A.; Štefánik, P.; Olexová, L.; Kršková, L. Testosterone enhancement during pregnancy influences social coping and gene expression of oxytocin and vasopressin in the brain of adult rats. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2018, 78, 264–270.
  25. Nuss, P. Anxiety disorders and GABA neurotransmission: A disturbance of modulation. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2015, 11, 165–175.
  26. Vázquez-León, P.; Miranda-Páez, A.; Valencia-Flores, K.; Sánchez-Castillo, H. Defensive and emotional behavior modulation by serotonin in the periaqueductal gray. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2023, 43, 1453–1468.
  27. Riskind, J.H.; Sagliano, L.; Trojano, L.; Conson, M. Dysfunctional Freezing Responses to Approaching Stimuli in Persons with a Looming Cognitive Style for Physical Threats. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 521.
  28. Riskind, J.H.; Black, D.; Shahar, G. Cognitive vulnerability to anxiety in the stress generation process: Interaction between the Looming Cognitive Style and Anxiety Sensitivity. J. Anxiety Disord. 2010, 24, 124–128.
  29. Darley, J.M.; Latané, B. Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1968, 8, 377–383. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0025589 (accessed on 10 October 2023).
  30. Latané, B.; Darley, J.M. The Unresponsive Bystander: Why Doesn’t He Help? Appleton Century Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1970.
  31. Kilpatrick, D.G.; Resnick, H.S.; Milanak, M.E.; Miller, M.W.; Keyes, K.M.; Friedman, M.J. National estimates of exposure to traumatic events and PTSD prevalence using DSM-IV and DSM-5 criteria. J. Trauma. Stress 2013, 26, 537–547.
  32. Carmassi, C.; Akiskal, H.S.; Yong, S.S.; Stratta, P.; Calderani, E.; Massimetti, E.; Akiskal, K.K.; Rossi, A.; Dell’Osso, L. Post-traumatic stress disorder in DSM-5: Estimates of prevalence and criteria comparison versus DSM-IV-TR in a non-clinical sample of earthquake survivors. J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 151, 843–848.
  33. Janson, G.R.; Hazler, R.J. Trauma reactions of bystanders and victims to repetitive abuse experiences. Violence Vict. 2004, 19, 239–255.
  34. Lazarus, R.S.; Folkman, S. Stress Appraisal and Coping; Springer Publishing Company: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1984.
  35. Nester, M.S.; Cook, M.H.; Pugach, C.P.; Wisco, B.E. Exploring false negatives in self-reported trauma exposure status. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy, 2024; in press.
  36. Rokach, A. (Ed.) The Psychology of Health and Illness; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2024.
  37. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Volume 5, No. 5.
  38. Bonanno, G.A.; Westphal, M.; Mancini, A.D. Resilience to loss and potential trauma. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2011, 7, 511–535.
  39. Berger, R. Stress, Trauma, and Posttraumatic Growth: Social Context, Environment, and Identities; Routledge: London, UK, 2015.
  40. Lauterbach, D.; Vrana, S. The relationship among personality variables, exposure to traumatic events, and severity of posttraumatic stress symptoms. J. Trauma. Stress 2001, 14, 29–45.
  41. Finkelhor, D.; Ormrod, R.K.; Turner, H.A. Polyvictimization and trauma in a national longitudinal cohort. Dev. Psychopathol. 2007, 19, 149–166.
  42. Yehuda, R. Risk and resilience in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2004, 65, 29–36.
  43. Center for Substance Abuse Treatment. Understanding the impact of trauma. In Trauma-Informed Care in Behavioral Health Services; Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (US): Rockville, MD, USA, 2014.
  44. Schimmenti, A. The trauma factor: Examining the relationships among different types of trauma, dissociation, and psychopathology. J. Trauma Dissociation 2018, 19, 552–571.
  45. Schimmenti, A.; Caretti, V. Linking the overwhelming with the unbearable: Developmental trauma, dissociation, and the disconnected self. Psychoanal. Psychol. 2016, 33, 106.
  46. Van der Kolk, B.A. The neurobiology of childhood trauma and abuse. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. 2003, 12, 293–317.
  47. Liotti, G. Trauma, dissociation, and disorganized attachment: Three strands of a single braid. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. Train. 2004, 41, 472.
  48. Van der Hart, O. Understanding trauma-generated dissociation and disorganised attachment: Giovanni Liotti’s lasting contributions. Attachment 2018, 12, 101–109.
  49. Schore, A.N. Relational trauma, brain development, and dissociation. In Treating Complex Traumatic Stress Disorders in Children and Adolescents: Scientific Foundations and Therapeutic Models; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 3–23.
  50. Perry, B.D.; Pollard, R. Homeostasis, stress, trauma, and adaptation: A neurodevelopmental view of childhood trauma. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. 1998, 7, 33–51.
  51. Finkelhor, D. Childhood Victimization: Violence, Crime, and Abuse in the Lives of Young People; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2008.
  52. Livia, D.; Petre, A. Effects of Bullying on Adolescent Suicidal Behavior. Glob. J. Arts Humanit. Soc. Sci. 2024, 12, 18–33.
  53. Vargas, T.G. Systemic Environmental Factors, Neural Correlates and Psychosis Vulnerability. Ph.D. Dissertation, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA, 2023.
  54. Garbarino, E.; Johnson, M.S. The Different Roles of Satisfaction, Trust, and Commitment in Customer Relationships. J. Mark. 1999, 63, 70–87.
  55. Richters, J.E.; Martinez, P. The NIMH community violence project: I. Children as victims of and witnesses to violence. Psychiatry 1993, 56, 7–21.
  56. Bremne, J.D.; Vermetten, E. Stress and development: Behavioral and biological consequences. Dev. Psychopathol. 2001, 13, 473–489.
  57. Boney-McCoy, S.; Finkelhor, D. Prior victimization: A risk factor for child sexual abuse and for PTSD-related symptomatology among sexually abused youth. Child Abus. Negl. 1995, 19, 1401–1421.
  58. Hazler, R.J.; Hoover, J.H.; Oliver, R. Student Perceptions of Victimization by Bullies in School. J. Humanist. Educ. Dev. 1991, 29, 143–150.
  59. Hoover, J.H.; Oliver, R.; Hazler, R.J. Bullying: Perceptions of adolescent victims in the Midwestern USA. Sch. Psychol. Int. 1992, 13, 5–16.
  60. Carney, J.V. Bullied to Death: Perceptions of Peer Abuse and Suicidal Behaviour during Adolescence. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2000, 21, 213–223.
  61. Hazler, R.J.; Carney, J.V. Empowering Peers to Prevent Youth Violence. J. Humanist. Couns. Educ. Dev. 2002, 41, 129–149.
  62. Fattah, E.A. Victims and Victimology: The Facts and the Rhetoric. Int. Rev. Vict. 1989, 1, 43–66.
  63. Brown, L.M.; Gilligan, C. Listening for voice in narratives of relationship. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 1991, 1991, 43–62.
  64. Safran, S.P.; Safran, J.S.; Barcikowski, R.S. Differences in Teacher Tolerance: An Illusory Phenomena? Behav. Disord. 1985, 11, 11–15.
  65. Itzkovich, Y.; Barhon, E.; Lev-Wiesel, R. Health and Risk Behaviors of Bystanders: An Integrative Theoretical Model of Bystanders’ Reactions to Mistreatment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5552.
  66. Knauf, R.; Eschenbeck, H.; Hock, M. Bystanders of bullying: Social-cognitive and affective reactions to school bullying and cyberbullying. Cyberpsychol. J. Psychosoc. Res. Cyberspace 2018, 12, 3.
  67. Bandura, A. Selective Moral Disengagement in the Exercise of Moral Agency. J. Moral Educ. 2002, 31, 101–119.
  68. Byers, D.S. Recognition of Social Pain among Peers: Rethinking the Role of Bystanders in Bullying and Cyberbullying. Smith Coll. Stud. Soc. Work 2016, 86, 335–354.
  69. Thornberg, R.; Jungert, T. Bystander behavior in bullying situations: Basic moral sensitivity, moral disengagement and defender self-efficacy. J. Adolesc. 2013, 36, 475–483.
  70. Wu, Y.; Zhou, Y.; Shi, L. Defender self-efficacy and moral disengagement on social support and bystander behaviors among primary school students: A multilevel moderated mediation model. Psicol. Refl. Crit. 2023, 36, 11.
  71. Hortensius, R.; de Gelder, B. From Empathy to Apathy: The Bystander Effect Revisited. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 27, 249–256.
  72. Gottlieb, A.; Schwartz, S. They Know I Saw It: Evaluation Apprehension and Diffusion of Responsibility in Bystander Reactions to a Violent Crime; ERIC: New Haven, CT, USA, 1976.
  73. Gray, J.A. The Psychology of Fear and Stress, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1988.
  74. Latané, B.; Nida, S. Ten years of research on group size and helping. Psychol. Bull. 1981, 89, 308–324.
  75. Fanselow, M.S. Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1994, 1, 429–438.
  76. Piper, C. Review of Child Victims: Crime, Impact and Criminal Justice, by J. Morgan & L. Zedner. Mod. Law Rev. 1993, 56, 611–613. Available online: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1096836 (accessed on 4 July 1993).
  77. Graziano, W.G.; Habashi, M.M. Motivational processes underlying both prejudice and helping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 14, 313–331.
  78. Leach, J. Cognitive paralysis in an emergency: The role of the supervisory attentional system. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2005, 76, 134–136.
  79. Schiller, D.; Alessandra, N.C.; Alia-Klein, N.; Becker, S.; Cromwell, H.C.; Dolcos, F.; Soreq, H. The human affectome. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2023, 2023, 105450.
  80. Bruno, A.; Rizzo, A.; Muscatello, M.R.A.; Celebre, L.; Silvestri, M.C.; Zoccali, R.A.; Mento, C. Hyperarousal scale: Italian cultural validation, age and gender differences in a nonclinical population. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1176.
  81. Xie, Z.; Liu, C.; Teng, Z. The effect of everyday moral sensitivity on bullying bystander behavior: Parallel mediating roles of empathy and moral disengagement. J. Interpers. Violence 2023, 38, 7678–7701.
  82. Muscatello, M.R.A.; Rizzo, A.; Celebre, L.; Mento, C.; Pandolfo, G.; Cedro, C.; Battaglia, F.; Zoccali, R.A.; Bruno, A. The wounds of childhood: Early trauma subtypes, salience and hyperarousal in a sample of adult psychiatric patients. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2020, 66, 3–9.
  83. Mento, C.; Rizzo, A.; Alfa, R.; Carlotta, V.; Lipari, E.; Bruno, A.; Cedro, C.; Pandolfo, G.; Muscatello, M.R.A.; Zoccali, R.A. The role of basic symptoms and aberrant salience in borderline personality disorder. J. Clin. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 2, 1–13.
  84. Çağiş, Z.G.; Akçe, İ.; Aziz, I.A.; Rizzo, A.; Nucera, G.; Bragazzi, N.; Yildirim, M. The mediating role of peritraumatic stress in the relationship between childhood psychological maltreatment and optimism and pessimism among earthquake survivors in Turkey. J. Health Soc. Sci. 2023, 8, 121–131.
  85. Kettrey, H.H.; Marx, R.A. The Effects of Bystander Programs on the Prevention of Sexual Assault across the College Years: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. J. Youth Adolesc. 2018, 48, 212–227.
  86. Bruno, A.; Mattei, A.; Arnone, F.; Barbieri, A.; Basile, V.; Cedro, C.; Celebre, L.; Mento, C.; Rizzo, A.; Silvestri, M.C.; et al. Lifetime psychiatric comorbidity and diagnostic trajectories in an Italian psychiatric sample. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 2020, 17, 263.
More
Information
Subjects: Psychology
Contributors MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register : , , , , , ,
View Times: 158
Revisions: 2 times (View History)
Update Date: 12 Mar 2024
1000/1000