Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 -- 1544 2023-07-11 18:28:48 |
2 layout Meta information modification 1544 2023-07-12 02:45:37 |

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Araya, C.; Coloma, C.J.; Quezada, C.; Benavente, P. Clause Complexity in Children with SLI/LDD. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/46656 (accessed on 28 April 2024).
Araya C, Coloma CJ, Quezada C, Benavente P. Clause Complexity in Children with SLI/LDD. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/46656. Accessed April 28, 2024.
Araya, Claudia, Carmen Julia Coloma, Camilo Quezada, Paula Benavente. "Clause Complexity in Children with SLI/LDD" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/46656 (accessed April 28, 2024).
Araya, C., Coloma, C.J., Quezada, C., & Benavente, P. (2023, July 11). Clause Complexity in Children with SLI/LDD. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/46656
Araya, Claudia, et al. "Clause Complexity in Children with SLI/LDD." Encyclopedia. Web. 11 July, 2023.
Clause Complexity in Children with SLI/LDD
Edit

Specific Language Disorder (SLI) or Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) is a developmental disorder characterized by a set of difficulties that affect language acquisition. This disorder manifests itself as an important limitation in the expression and/or comprehension of oral language, which affects communicative practices in speech and language processing.

grammar clause complexity longitudinal study specific language impairment (SLI)

1. Syntactic Complexity in Spanish-Speaking Children with SLI/DLD

Several studies on sentence complexity across different languages indicate that it poses a significant challenge for children with SLI/DLD [1][2][3][4][5][6][7][8]. Research on Spanish-speaking children with SLI/DLD has found that they exhibit difficulties in both sentence comprehension and production, particularly in the area of complex sentences [1][2][9]. Moreover, production difficulties seem to be more pronounced as sentence complexity increases [10]. These challenges are also more prevalent in contexts that require the use of more sophisticated linguistic resources, such as narrative discourse production [11][12].

Simple and Complex Structures

Many studies on morphosyntactic complexity in children with SLI/DLD have focused on analyzing their language samples in terms of simple sentences and those related to coordination and subordination mechanisms. It has been found that compared to coordinated sentences, children with SLI/DLD have fewer problems with simple sentences [10]. Furthermore, it has been suggested that these children tend to use simple sentences more frequently than complex ones [13] and prefer using simple and coordinated sentences over complex sentences [2][5][10][14].
The analysis of syntactic complexity in children with SLI/DLD considers different perspectives on the concept of complexity. Some studies include coordinated sentences in their description of compound sentences, which they consider as constitutive of complexity. In this regard, it has been observed that children with SLI/DLD use significantly fewer compound sentences than their typically developing peers [1]. Other studies focus only on subordination, which involves integrating one clause within another [13][15][16]. From this perspective, it has been found that children with SLI/DLD have lower production of subordinate clauses than typically developing children [2][4][9][17][18]. However, some studies have not found these differences to be statistically significant, at least in narrative language samples [13][16].

2. Longitudinal Development and Trajectory of Syntactic Complexity in Children with Typical Development (TD) and with SLI/DLD

The use of subordinate clauses as a mechanism of syntactic complexity in children’s linguistic development highlights their ability to produce sentences that are dependent on others [19]. Typically developing children begin to use two or more verbs in a sentence around 2 years of age, with complex syntax emerging around 30 months; however, the structures and functions involved in complexity are not fully consolidated until after three years of age [7][16]. The developmental trajectory among preschool children with typical development demonstrates substantial and noteworthy variations. Despite these variations, children demonstrate a certain degree of complexity in their statements before the age of four [20]. However, coordinated clauses remain prevalent during the preschool age [7]. By 6 years of age, they handle syntactically more complex linguistic structures than in preschool years [21], although simple sentences are still used more frequently than complex sentences. In the first years of schooling, from age 6 onwards, the use of subordination increases significantly [22]. Finally, studies indicate that typically developing children use more complex clauses at 10 years than at 8 years [23].
On the other hand, children with SLI/DLD preferably use simple sentences between 4 and 6 years of age [5]. However, certain indicators of complex syntax have also been observed in a longitudinal case study involving a child at the age of 5 years and 9 months [8]. It has also been observed that these children use significantly fewer compound sentences in their narratives between the ages of 4 to 11 [1]. Furthermore, Pavez et al. [24] and Coloma et al. [25] showed that, at 6 years of age, children with SLI/DLD maintained the same level of production of complex structures as a control group of 4-year-olds in narrative samples. Regarding conversation, it has been suggested that both children with SLI/DLD and those with language delay produce fewer complex structures than their typically developing peers [4][26]. In this regard, Hincapié-Henao et al. [9] state that children with SLI/DLD have great difficulty in producing complex verbally formulated structures. Among these structures, constructions that reflect hypotactic relationships with time-related, final, and comparative value are also especially challenging [10].
Describing the development of language longitudinally allows, on the one hand, to determine different growth patterns or trajectories that could define characteristics of typical and atypical language [8][27][28] and, on the other, to investigate in greater depth the issue of the persistence of SLI/DLD difficulties over time [29]. Law and Tomblin [28] mention three hypotheses that explain the possible development patterns of language skills in children with SLI/DLD: (1) they coincide at the same starting point and diverge over time (“deterioration hypothesis”); (2) they develop at the same speed, but stop at a certain point, without further development (“plateau hypothesis”); and (3) they take off later, but their language development, although delayed, parallels that of typical development (“tracking hypothesis”). According to Law and Tomblin [28], a reduced heterogeneity is observed in the growth characteristics of children with language disorders, a trajectory that would be similar to that of children with typical language development, at least in the school years, which would be consistent with the explanation of the tracking hypothesis. This finding coincides with the position that, although these children would be delayed, they would not be qualitatively different from children with TD [30]. In this regard, it has also been suggested that children with TD and SLI/DLD would follow a similar path, although children with SLI/DLD obtain lower results [29]. However, it is necessary to emphasize, at this point, that the competence and speed of development in children with SLI/DLD is lower than in children with typical development with respect to the emergence of syntactic complexity at the beginning of these trajectories [15][25] and that the specific characteristics of this starting point provide evidence that widespread vulnerabilities in complex syntax acquisition could typify SLI/DLD [8].

3. Approaches for the Description of Syntactic Complexity in Children with TD and SLI/DLD

Various approaches have been utilized to assess complexity in children with typical development and SLI/DLD. These methods offer distinct viewpoints and outcomes, with some focusing on quantitative aspects and others on qualitative aspects. Quantitatively, syntactic complexity has been evaluated based on the average length of specific units, such as sentences, clauses, or utterances. Brown [31] proposed the Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) measurement for analyzing children’s language in early developmental stages, which is systematically related to age and accounts for the development of syntactic maturity [32]. This index has been used in studies on various languages, including Spanish [33], English [34], and Portuguese [35], in children with TD and SLI/DLD. The MLU has been linked to other measures of complexity, enabling researchers to observe that older children produce longer linguistic units containing more clauses [19]. Combining the MLU with other indices of ungrammaticality has facilitated the identification of children with language difficulties [36]. Additionally, measuring the MLU in bilingual children with TD (English–Spanish) has been predictive of their language skills in an English narrative retelling test, although the same result was not obtained in Spanish [37].
An alternative approach to measuring syntactic complexity involves focusing on clauses and their relationships, as proposed by Hunt [38][39] (1965, 1970). According to this generativist perspective, a terminal unit (T-unit) is composed of a main clause and any attached or embedded clauses or non-clausal structures, representing both paratactic (juxtaposed and coordinated clauses) and hypotactic (subordinate clauses) relationships. This method allows for the quantitative growth of T-units to be visualized as a child’s development becomes more complex, with indices increasing in parallel with age, schooling, and intellectual level [39].
Describing language samples of children with SLI/DLD has frequently employed the quantification and structural analysis of simple, coordinated, and subordinate sentences as a means of characterizing language complexity [1][2][9][13][15][16][17]. Within this tradition, sentence complexity is typically interpreted based on the presence or absence of different types of sentences, with complexity often referring to utterances that exceed the limits of a clause [16].
Another approach to measuring language complexity involves a qualitative analysis of language samples, focusing not only on syntax, but also on thematic and discursive criteria. In this tradition, researchers have analyzed the distribution and organization of clauses based on interclausal relationships, which can be classified as isotactic, symmetrical and asymmetrical paratactic, hypotactic, and endotactic. For example, Alfaro, Crespo, and Alvarado [14] studied these relationships in a narrative sample of children with SLI/DLD and typically developing children. They found that the SLI/DLD group produced more paratactic relationships than the TD group, although this difference was not statistically significant. This finding suggests that the SLI/DLD group produced less informative texts, indicating lower complexity.
From a systemic–functional perspective, the interpretation of the role of interdependence between clauses (parataxis and hypotaxis) is different. In this approach, clauses, which are considered as the central unit of grammatical meaning, are capable of expressing different types of meaning simultaneously and their function is explained by how they work together [40]. The logical system of “taxis” (from the Greek: order, arrangement, and category) captures the relationship of dependency and interdependence between adjacent clauses, which may be potentially part of different types of clause complexes [41]. At the semantic level, clause complexes illustrate how a flow of events develops and becomes a text [42]. According to this theoretical perspective, differences in the structure of oral or written texts reflect differences of a semantic nature [40][43].

References

  1. Acosta, V.M.; Axpe, Á.; Moreno, A.M. El estudio de la agramaticalidad en el discurso narrativo del trastorno específico del lenguaje. Onomázein 2014, 29, 119–129.
  2. Buiza, J.J.; Adrián, J.A.; González, M.; Rodríguez-Parra, M.J. Evaluación de marcadores psicolingüísticos en el diagnóstico de niños con trastorno específico del lenguaje. Rev. de Logop. Foniatr. y Audiol. 2004, 24, 142–155.
  3. Leonard, L.B.; Wong, A.; Deevy, P.; Stokes, S.F.; Fletcher, P. The production of passives by children with specific language impairment: Acquiring English or Cantonese. Appl. Psychoinguist. 2006, 27, 267–299.
  4. Marinellie, S.A. Complex syntax used by school-age children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) in child-adult conversation. J. Commun. Disord. 2004, 37, 517–533.
  5. Moreno, A. La intervención en morfosintaxis desde un enfoque interactivo: Un estudio de escolares con retraso de lenguaje. Rev. de Logop. Foniatr. y Audiol. 2003, 23, 89–97.
  6. Schuele, C.M.; Tolbert, L. Omissions of obligatory relative markers in children with specific language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2001, 15, 257–274.
  7. Serra, M.; Serrat, E.; Solé, R.; Bel, A.; Aparici, M. La Adquisición del Lenguaje, 1st ed.; Ariel: Barcelona, Spain, 2000.
  8. Schuele, C.M.; Dykes, J.C. Complex syntax acquisition: A longitudinal case study of a child with specific language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2005, 19, 295–318.
  9. Hincapié, L.; Giraldo, M.; Lopera, F.; Pineda, D.; Castro, R.; Lopera, J.P.; Mendieta, N.; Jaramillo, A.; Arboleda, A.; Aguirre, D.; et al. Trastorno Específico del Desarrollo del Lenguaje en una población infantil colombiana. Univ. Psychol. 2008, 7, 557–569.
  10. Buiza, J.; Rodríguez-Parra, M.J.; González-Sánchez, M.; Adrián, J. Specific language impairment: Evaluation and detection of differential psycholinguistic markers in phonology and morphosyntax in Spanish-speaking children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 58, 65–82.
  11. Auzá, A.; Chávez, A. Medidas del desarrollo del lenguaje en niños mexicanos: La longitud media de emisión, diversidad y densidad léxicas en el recuento de una historia. In ¿Qué Me Cuentas? Narraciones y Desarrollo Lingüístico en Niños Hispanohablantes, 1st ed.; Auza, A., Hess, K., Eds.; Ediciones De Laurel: Monterrey, Mexico, 2013; pp. 59–84.
  12. Thordardottir, E. Language-specific effects of task demands on the manifestation of specific language impairment: A comparison of English and Icelandic. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2008, 51, 922–937.
  13. Coloma, C.J.; Araya, C.; Quezada, C.; Pavez, M.M.; Maggiolo, M. Grammaticality and complexity of sentences in monolingual Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2016, 30, 649–662.
  14. Alfaro, P.; Crespo, N.; Alvarado, C. Complejidad sintáctica en narraciones de niños con desarrollo típico, trastorno específico del lenguaje y discapacidad intelectual. Sintag. Rev. Ling. 2016, 28, 27–41.
  15. Barako Arndt, K.; Schuele, C.M. Production of infinitival complements by children with specific language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2012, 26, 1–17.
  16. Jackson-Maldonado, D.; Maldonado, R. La complejidad sintáctica en niños con y sin Trastorno Primario de Lenguaje. In Lingüística Funcional, 1st ed.; Rodríguez Sánchez, I., Vázquez, E., Eds.; Universidad Autónoma de Querétaro: Querétaro, México, 2015; pp. 253–301.
  17. Ferinú, L.; Ahufinger, N.; Pacheco-Vera, F.; Andreu, L.; Sanz-Torrent, M. Dificultades morfosintácticas en niños de 5 a 8 años con trastorno del desarrollo del lenguaje a través de subpruebas del CELF-4. Rev. Logop. Foniatr. Audiol. 2021, 41, 17–28.
  18. Scott, C.; Windsor, J. General Language Performance Measures in Spoken and Written Narratives and Expository Discourse of School-Age Children with Language Learning Disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2000, 43, 324–339.
  19. Castilla-Earls, A.P.; Eriks-Brophy, A. Spontaneous language measures in monolingual preschool Spanish-speaking children. Rev. Logop. Foniatr. Audiol. 2012, 32, 97–108.
  20. Clemente, R.A. Desarrollo del Lenguaje, 1st ed.; Octaedro: Barcelona, Spain, 2000.
  21. Auzá, A.; Alarcón, L. Cláusulas subordinadas y coordinadas en dos tareas narrativas producidas por niños mexicanos de primero de primaria. In Proceedings of the XVI Congreso Internacional de la ALFAL, Alcalá de Henares, Spain, 6–9 June 2011.
  22. Gutierrez-Clellen, V.F.; Hofstetter, R. Syntactic complexity in Spanish narratives: A developmental study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1994, 37, 645–654.
  23. Coloma, C.J.; Peñaloza, C.; Fernández, R. Producción de oraciones complejas en niños de 8 y 10 años. RLA Rev. Ling. Teor. Apl. 2007, 45, 33–44.
  24. Pavez, M.; Coloma, C.J.; Araya, C.; Maggiolo, M.; Peñaloza, C. Gramaticalidad y complejidad en narración y conversación en niños con trastorno específico del lenguaje. Rev. Logop. Foniatr. Audiol. 2015, 35, 150–158.
  25. Coloma, C.J.; Araya, C.; Quezada, C.; Pavez, M.; Álvarez, C.; Maggiolo, M. Development of grammaticality and sentence complexity in monolingual Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment: An exploratory study. Sintag. Rev. Ling. 2019, 31, 87–101.
  26. Domsch, C.; Richels, C.; Saldana, M.; Coleman, C.; Wimberly, C.; Maxwell, L. Narrative skill and syntactic complexity in school-age children with and without late language emergence. Int. J. Lang Commun. Disord. 2012, 47, 197–207.
  27. Leonard, L. Children with Specific Language Impairment, 1st ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1998.
  28. Law, J.; Tomblin, B.; Zhang, X. Characterizing the Growth Trajectories of Language-Impaired Children Between 7 and 11 Years of Age. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2008, 51, 739–749.
  29. Crespo Allende, N.; Alfaro-Faccio, P.; Góngora-Costa, B.; Alvarado, C.; Marfull-Villanueva, D. Perfil sintáctico de niños con y sin Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje: Un análisis descriptivo. Rev. Signos. 2020, 53, 619–642.
  30. Rice, M.L. Growth models of developmental language disorders. In Developmental Language Disorders, 1st ed.; Rice, M.L., Ed.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2004; pp. 214–247.
  31. Brown, R. A First Language: The Early Stages, 1st ed.; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1973.
  32. Checa, I. Madurez sintáctica y subordinación: Los índices secundarios clausales. Interlingüística 2003, 14, 191–206.
  33. Coloma, C.J.; Silva, M.; Palma, S.; Holtheuer, C. Reading Comprehension in Children with Specific Language Impairment: An Exploratory Study of Linguistic and Decoding Skills. Psykhe 2015, 24, 1–8.
  34. Rice, M.; Smolik, F.; Perpich, D.; Thompson, T.; Rytting, N.; Blossom, M. Mean length of utterance levels in 6-month intervals for children 3 to 9 years with and without language impairments. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2010, 53, 333–349.
  35. Santos, M.; Lynce, S.; Carvalho, S.; Cacela, M.; Mineiro, A. Extensão média do enunciado-palavras em crianças de 4 e 5 años com desenvolvimento típico da linguagem. Rev. CEFAC 2015, 17, 1143–1151.
  36. Simon-Cereijido, G.; Gutierrez-Clellen, V.F. Spontaneous language markers of Spanish language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2007, 28, 317–339.
  37. Bedore, L.M.; Pena, E.D.; Gillam, R.B.; Ho, T.H. Language sample measures and language ability in Spanish-English bilingual kindergarteners. J. Commun. Disord. 2010, 43, 498–510.
  38. Hunt, K.W. Grammatical Structures Written at Three Grade Levels. NCTE Research Report No. 3; 1965. Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED113735 (accessed on 2 June 2023).
  39. Hunt, K.W. Recent measures in syntactic development. Elem. Engl. 1966, 43, 732–739.
  40. Halliday, M.A.K.; Matthiessen, C.M. Introduction to Functional Grammar, 1st ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2004.
  41. Eggins, S. Introduction to Systemic Functional Linguistics, 1st ed.; Ayc Black: London, UK, 2004.
  42. Halliday, M.A.K.; Webster, J. The Language of Early Childhood, 1st ed.; Bloomsbury: London, UK, 2004.
  43. Thompson, G. Introducing Functional Grammar, 1st ed.; Arnold: London, UK, 1996.
More
Information
Subjects: Others
Contributors MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register : , , ,
View Times: 255
Revisions: 2 times (View History)
Update Date: 12 Jul 2023
1000/1000