Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 -- 3577 2023-01-19 18:23:01 |
2 format Meta information modification 3577 2023-01-29 03:48:28 |

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Goldberg, H.; , H. Educational Neuroscience. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/40423 (accessed on 08 May 2024).
Goldberg H,  H. Educational Neuroscience. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/40423. Accessed May 08, 2024.
Goldberg, Hagar, Hagar . "Educational Neuroscience" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/40423 (accessed May 08, 2024).
Goldberg, H., & , H. (2023, January 19). Educational Neuroscience. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/40423
Goldberg, Hagar and Hagar . "Educational Neuroscience." Encyclopedia. Web. 19 January, 2023.
Educational Neuroscience
Edit

Compared to other primates, humans are late bloomers, with exceptionally long childhood and adolescence. The extensive developmental period of humans is thought to facilitate the learning processes required for the growth and maturation of the complex human brain. During the first two and a half decades of life, the human brain is a construction site, and learning processes direct its shaping through experience-dependent neuroplasticity. Formal and informal learning, which generates long-term and accessible knowledge, is mediated by neuroplasticity to create adaptive structural and functional changes in brain networks. Since experience-dependent neuroplasticity is at full force during school years, it holds a tremendous educational opportunity. In order to fulfill this developmental and learning potential, educational practices should be human-brain-friendly and “ride” the neuroplasticity wave. Neuroscience can inform educators about the natural learning mechanisms of the brain to support student learning.

educational neuroscience neuroplasticity human learning and development

1. Introduction

Educational neuroscience is an interdisciplinary field exploring the effects of education on the human brain and promotes the translation of research findings to brain-based pedagogies and policies [1]. The brain is the target organ of education. Education is thought to influence brain development [2][3] and health, even as the brain ages [4][5]. Studying the dynamics between the brain and education can be instrumental in finding ways to better support learners across the lifespan.
Educational neuroscience research explores every possible relationship between the physiological, mental, and behavioral aspects of learning. Some studies have tried to identify the optimal physical conditions for neuroplasticity and learning. This stream of educational neuroscience research includes studies exploring the effects of sleep (or sleep deprivation), physical exercise, and environmental pollution on the brain and its cognitive performance [1]. While these studies focus on the effect of brain health on learning, other studies examine the effect of learning on brain health, assessing the long-term effects of learning/education on the human brain and exploring in what ways formal/informal education is associated with better aging of the human brain [2][3][4].
Some educational neuroscience studies take a developmental approach to study the relationship between cognitive and learning capacities across the lifespan. For example, multilevel measurements collected from adolescents (e.g., neuronal, hormonal, psychological, and behavioral) have advanced understanding of how the massive neuronal changes that take place during adolescence promote cognitive development but also introduce immense neuronal and mental vulnerability (and the onset of most psychiatric disorders) [1][5][6][7]. Other studies in this line of research explore the factors supporting neuroplasticity in the mature brain—to support lifelong learning [8].
Educational neuroscience also explores the nature–nurture aspects of learning, for example, examining how learning environments interact with genetic conditions and what DNA variations predict differential learning abilities [9]. Environmental influences on learning include studies about the impacts of socio-economic status (SES) on the brain and cognitive developmental trajectory [10]. Furthermore, educational neuroscience seeks to understand the mechanisms that facilitate general learning abilities (such as executive control and social and emotional skills), discipline-specific learning abilities (such as literacy, numeracy, and science), the connections between these mechanisms, and the extent to which these learning skills are trainable [11].
As a developing, interdisciplinary research field, educational neuroscience faces challenges, limitations, and criticism, especially concerning the ability to generalize research findings in lab conditions to classroom learning, and its validity and transferability to larger scales, such as mass education systems. Other challenges stem from the fact that learning is one of the most basic yet complex brain functions that incorporates the entire brain and has a continuous effect. Furthermore, empirical studies in educational neuroscience are challenging and cumbersome due to the interdisciplinary nature of the field (education, psychology, and neuroscience); the need for repeated measures over time; and the young target population (school students), which imposes ethical restrictions on experimental designs. Finally, while still evolving as a research field, educational neuroscience is intriguing for many educational leaders who are enthusiastic about applying neuroscience in education practices. Unfortunately, the current gap between the high demand and limited supply may lead to misuse of neuroscience in pedagogy (e.g., neuromyths or the justification of educational methods based on limited to no evidence) [1].
While educational neuroscience is preliminary in forming evidence-based pedagogy, it can already offer valuable information and a much-needed bridge between educators and scientists in translating the research of learning into effective educational practices.
Neuroscience-informed educational design (teaching the way the brain learns) can promote learning motivation, high-level information processing, and knowledge retention. Moreover, neuroscience educational content (teaching about the brain) can inform students about their developing brains to promote scientific education and self-exploration.

2. Learning and Neuroplasticity

Human development is based on nature (genetics), nurture (physical and social environments), and their interactions (epigenetics) [12][13]. These factors play an essential role in learning processes and the reorganization of neuronal networks to create neuronal representations of new knowledge. Learning and training new knowledge or skills evoke specific and repeated activity patterns, and in the process of Hebbian neuroplasticity, neural pathways are reinforced by the strengthening of specific synapses, while less functional ones are eliminated [14][15][16].
Almost half a century ago, Vygotsky introduced the zone of proximal development (ZPD) [17] in education. According to the ZPD, learning and development depend on an optimal balance between support and challenge (the zone of proximal development and neuroplasticity), which should be tuned and tailored for each learner based on their specific developmental stage. The ZPD model was revolutionary, as it emphasized the importance of the educational environment (nurture) in unlocking the internal potential (nature) of students, and it placed the learning process (as opposed to the learning product) as the central educational goal [17]. Some decades later, the biology of learning revealed a beautiful alignment with Vygotsky’s theory—with evidence showing that brain neuroplasticity is highly affected by environmental conditions and the balance between demands (challenge) and available resources (support) [18]. The impact of stressors on learning can be constructive or destructive depending on the intensity, duration, and accumulation of the stressors and the coping mechanisms and support that one has.
Neuroscience research suggests that experience-dependent neuroplasticity [19], which facilitates learning processes, benefits from several principles. The central one is that learning a skill or new knowledge requires the activation of relevant neuronal pathways. The research also points to the saliency, intensity, and repetition of the learned skill/knowledge as valuable strategies for enhancing neuroplastic changes [16][20][21]. Learners cannot be passive recipients of content but must be active participants in the learning process.
An enriched environment for enhanced neuroplasticity offers physiological integrity, cognitive challenge, and emotional safety. More specifically, an enriched environment includes adequate sleep and nutrition, sensory–motor and cognitive challenges, opportunities for exploration and novelty, and secured relationships that act like a safety net and enable learners to take on challenges [22][23]. Conversely, a lack of these conditions may slow down or decrease the level of neuroplasticity in the developing brain.
The social and cognitive safety net that enables learners to aim high while taking risks and to turn failure into resilience is rooted in safe relationships (with adults and peers) and in holding a growth mindset. A growth mindset is the belief that intelligence and learning potential are not fixed and can be developed [24]. Holding a growth mindset has been associated with academic success, emotional wellbeing, and motivation while reducing racial, gender, and social class achievement gaps [25][26][27][28][29][30]. While the impact of mindset interventions on academic performance is debatable regarding the general population [31], the literature is clear about the potential of growth mindset intervention in supporting the academic development of high-risk and economically disadvantaged students [26][27][31][32].
The notion of human potential as something dynamic resonates with the concept of the plastic brain. Moreover, teaching students about neuroplasticity and the dynamic potential of their brains has been shown to effectively reinforce a growth mindset [32].

2.1. Using Neuroplasticity as Educational Content

Teaching students about experience-based neuroplasticity and the dynamic changes in neuronal networks during learning provides strong evidence of their natural and powerful learning capacity. Furthermore, teaching students about neuroplasticity with explicit connections to the growth mindset and development creates a motivating premise for learners—according to which their learning potential is dynamic and depends significantly on their attitudes and learning practices.
The neuroplasticity rules of “use it or lose it” and “use it to improve it” mean that, while teachers should support and guide them, learning occurs by and within the students. This physiology-based realization can help build students’ responsibility and ownership over their learning.
Harnessing neuroplasticity and a growth mindset to motivate students can be especially important with neurodivergent learners, whose cognitive development and learning styles deviate from the typical range. Twenty percent of the population is neurodivergent, including students on the autistic spectrum (ASD), students with learning disabilities (e.g., dyslexia), attention disorders (e.g., ADHD), neurological disorders (e.g., epilepsy), and mental illness (e.g., PTSD). While neurodiversity and variations in neuronal and cognitive expressions hold many advantages [33], neurodivergent students face extra challenges navigating neurotypical-oriented school systems. Learning about neuroplasticity can be a potent form of validation for neurodivergent students, as neurodiversity is a natural result of experience-dependent neuroplasticity [19]. In addition, by fostering a growth mindset and neuroplasticity awareness, neurodivergent students can be motivated to participate in evidence-based interventions. For example, teaching students with dyslexia about the specific structural and functional brain changes associated with the reading interventions that they apply [34][35][36] can motivate them to endure the hard work before noticing visible results.

2.2. Using Neuroplasticity to Guide Learning Design

Organizing learning systems around conditions that promote neuroplasticity can enhance learners’ academic development and wellbeing. When a student accomplishes today what was not in their reach yesterday, it is the product of neuroplasticity through a growth mindset.
Educational environments that promote neuroplasticity include encouraging and modeling a healthy lifestyle (physical exercise, a balanced diet, sufficient sleep, and regulated stress), —for example, educating students about the counter-productiveness of sleep deprivation (e.g., “all-nighter” study marathons) on learning. In addition, learning systems should invest in intellectual stimulation (novelty and challenge) and the system’s social and emotional climate (human connections). Neuroplasticity and development are optimal in the stretch zone, where learners experience a motivating level of challenge and stimulation while feeling emotionally supported and socially safe. This ratio between support and challenge should be individualized (between learners and within learners over time).
Educating teachers about neuroplasticity can be powerful in understanding and supporting students that were affected by trauma. Childhood adversity hampers neuroplasticity duration and magnitude [37]; a surviving brain is not a learning brain. While neuroplasticity is compromised by early trauma, neuroplasticity is also the key to healing from trauma. Schools have a pivotal role in battling the damage of early trauma by creating enriched and safe learning environments that reinforce alternative neuronal pathways to reverse the effects of early adverse environments on child brain development [22][38][39][40].

3. Learning Motivation and Reward

Learning and adaptation are essential for surviving and thriving in dynamic environments. The brain evolved to make sense of information from external and internal environments and to produce adaptive behaviors that promote survival. The brain is, therefore, a learning machine by nature, and learning does not require external initiation. However, learning is highly experience-dependent and can be directed and enhanced through education.
The brain reward system evolved to reinforce effortful behaviors that are essential for survival (e.g., foraging, reproduction, and caregiving). Such behaviors activate the dopaminergic system associated with reward and motivation [41]. The hormone/neurotransmitter dopamine is a central player in reward-motivated behavior and learning through the modulation of striatal and prefrontal functions [42]. The human brain reward system balances between (limbic) impulsive desire and (cortical) goal-directed wanting to guide flexible decision-making and adaptive motivational behaviors.
Psychologically, intrinsic motivation is driven by the need to experience a sense of competence, self-determination, and relatedness [43][44][45][46][47].
Competence refers to a perception of self-efficacy and confidence in one’s abilities to achieve a valuable outcome. Self-determination refers to the sense of autonomy and agency in the learning process. Relatedness refers to the drive to pursue goals that hold social value, which can be achieved by working collaboratively as part of a team or by creating something that resonates with others. Relatedness is a strong motivational driver, as it touches on a primary and primordial need to be part of a group and a higher spiritual and intellectual need for self-transcendence and impact.
Overall, these components are based on the human inclination to be valued and validated by the self and others. Biologically, they reflect basic survival needs that combine self-reliance (competence and ownership) and social reliance. Psychologically, these are all subjective perceptions that serve the need to maintain positive self-perception and self-integration. Finally, educationally, they reflect the natural human curiosity and tendency to learn and develop continuously.
The human brain reward system in the 21st century is an evolutionary mismatch. There is a discrepancy between the conditions that the reward system evolved to serve and those that it often faces in the 21st century. The reward system evolved over millions of years to motivate humans to work hard (invest time and energy) in maintaining their survival needs (e.g., nutrition, protection, reproduction, and the learning of new skills). However, this system is not designed for the abundance and immediacy of stimulation in the digital and instant reward era, which promotes the persistent release of dopamine that leads to an increased craving for reward (seeking behavior; wanting) and a decreased sense of pleasure and satisfaction (liking) [42][48].
Some of the most significant challenges of modern education systems relate to the massive changes in how people consume information and communicate in the digital era. Digital platforms have become dominant in information consumption and communication, which provide access to unlimited information and reinforce immediate rewards.

3.1. Using Neuroscience (of Reward and Motivation) as Educational Content

The science of human motivation, including its evolutionary mismatch, can be utilized to shed some light on students’ struggles with learning motivation. It can further provide a framework for students to explore their motivational (approach or avoid) tendencies regarding learning and academic challenges. Moreover, learning the neuroscience underlying motivation and reward can raise students’ awareness and proactivity in managing and protecting their reward system. Since adolescence is the peak time for the initiation of substance use, and early onset imposes a higher risk of mental health and substance abuse disorders persisting into adulthood [49][50][51], neuroscience knowledge about the reward system and its vulnerability (especially during brain development) is essential educational knowledge that can help in the prevention and mitigation of teen addiction.

3.2. Using Neuroscience to Guide Learning Design and Intrinsic Motivation

While students of the digital era are the most stimulation-flooded and attention-challenged in human history, learning is a process that takes time, selective attention, and perseverance. Therefore, learning designs that harness students’ intrinsic motivation for training and the development of stamina and grit (skills that might be hampered in the digital era) are precious for students’ health and success.
Motivational drivers include an adequate level of challenge that fits the student’s sense of competence and that creates optimal arousal levels, opportunities to expand social relatedness and impact, and balance between support and autonomy (see the ZPD).
Importantly, in classroom learning, educators are required to manage the attention, motivation, and reward system of not one but many students, which is a complex task. The typical classroom presents a broad spectrum of learners with diverse learning needs and stretch zones. While the facilitation of autonomy and the sense of competence varies between learners and requires personalized support, the social norms that promote learning are more ubiquitous and apply to most learners. While educators do not always have the resources to support students’ motivation individually, harnessing the social aspects of classroom learning is a manageable, effective strategy to elevate students’ motivation. Learning environments that demonstrate empathy, inclusiveness, and psychological safety have shown positive results in students’ behavior, self-esteem, motivation, and academic success [52][53][54][55][56]. Social motivation has been shown to enhance the encoding of new information (even if the content is not social) [57]. Learning-for-teaching and peer tutoring (one student teaching another student) effectively encode information into memory. Beyond memory improvement, peer tutoring has many further benefits to both the tutor and the learner in academic achievements [58][59], motivation, and ownership over the learning process and results in a deep conceptual understanding of the material [60].
The teacher’s demeanor is another controllable factor with a high potential to affect students’ motivation. For example, the literature points to teachers’ immediacy (creating physical and psychological closeness with students) as an effective way to enhance students’ engagement, learning motivation, and performance (including memory retention) [61][62][63][64][65]. Immediacy can be demonstrated through verbal and non-verbal gestures that communicate interest and personal connection (relating to personal stories, using animated voice and body language, creating eye contact, and using humor).
The research also indicates that, when students perceive the content as being personally relevant, they are more motivated to study [66]. Therefore, educators can actively make the learning content more relevant by using stories and real-life examples, making explicit connections and demonstrations of how the content may be relevant/applicable to the students, and giving students opportunities to reflect and share their connections to the learning material.

4. Intrinsic and Extrinsic Processing in Learning and Meaning-Making

As the environment provides more information than the brain can handle, survival depends on saliency detection and attention management to direct perception and behavior. The brain constantly selects and attends to relevant input while suppressing irrelevant or distracting information [67]. Information that is valuable or urgent for survival and prosperity receives attention. Attention capacities (e.g., alerting, orienting, and controlling attention) are managed by several brain systems that interact and coordinate [68][69][70]. Top–down, cognitive-driven attention that fosters a goal-directed thinking process is associated with the dorsal attention network (consisting of the intraparietal sulcus and the frontal eye fields) [71]. This mechanism enables students to read a paragraph, listen to a lecture, think about the teacher’s question, or write an essay. A second attention system is bottom–up and stimulus-driven, and it orients attention to unexpected and behaviorally relevant stimuli. This ventral attention network consists of the right temporoparietal junction and the ventral frontal cortex [71]. This attention-grabbing mechanism enables the individual to respond quickly to urgent environmental demands, for example, moving away to prevent a struck-by-object accident. Flexible attention control depends on dynamic interactions and switching between the two systems and involves the central executive network (CEN) [68][70].
The insula and anterior cingulate cortex comprise the core structures of the saliency network [72], another major player in attention altering to emotionally salient stimuli through the interaction of the sensory and cognitive influences that control attention [72][73][74].
In addition to outward-focused attention, the human brain is also invested in inward-focused processing. Functional brain imaging studies of the human brain show a robust functional anticorrelation between two large-scale systems, one highly extrinsic and the other deeply intrinsic [75][76][77]. The central executive network (CEN) is an externally driven system and is paramount for attention control, working memory, flexible thinking, and goal-directed behavior. The core components of the CEN are the dorsolateral PFC and the lateral posterior parietal cortex (hence, the frontoparietal network) [72][78]. When the human brain is not occupied with external tasks, the default mode network (DMN) is activated. This internally driven cognitive network includes the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) as core components. The DMN is thought to facilitate reminiscing, contemplating, autobiographical memory, self-reflecting, and social cognition [79]. Conversely, the DMN is immediately suppressed when the brain is engaged in externally driven tasks and stimulation.
Resting-state brain imaging studies revealed that the activity in the DMN during resting awake states indicates the quality of subsequent neural and behavioral responses to environmental stimuli [72][80]. Moreover, a high connectivity between “intrinsic” (DMN) and “extrinsic” (CEN) brain networks, and specifically emotional saliency, attention (extrinsic), and reflection (intrinsic) networks is associated with better cognitive performance, meaning-making, and broad perspective thinking [75][76][81]. These networks function antagonistically but are highly connected and balance each other. Furthermore, the anticorrelation between their function is associated with better task performance and positive mental health [79][82]. Recent studies also suggest that a causal hierarchical architecture orchestrates this anticorrelation between externally and internally driven brain activities. More specifically, that regions of the saliency network and the dorsal attention network impose inhibition on the DMN. Conversely, the DMN exhibits an excitatory influence on the saliency and attention system [79].

4.1. The Neuroscience of Extrinsic and Intrinsic Processing as Educational Content

Teaching students about the dynamics of the default mode and executive control network can help them understand how their brain processes information, the importance of each process (e.g., extrinsic and intrinsic), and their integration for meaningful learning. This knowledge can be applied as students explore and experiment with ways to enhance their learning and memory by intentionally engaging both intrinsic and extrinsic processing and integrating the two.

4.2. Using the Neuroscience of Extrinsic and Intrinsic Processing to Guide Learning Design

Traditionally, instructional education is based on learning objectives that are externally dictated and is focused on outward attention (stimulus-driven lectures and assignments). Mind-wandering has become the enemy of classroom teachers, as it indicates students’ lack of attention and poor learning.
Nevertheless, neuroscience research indicates that meaning-making and cognitive performance benefit from the interplay between extrinsic and intrinsic oriented attention and processing [56][75].
Learning instructions should consider the different attention mechanisms, evoking adequate arousal levels and leading to goal-directed thinking. Furthermore, students will benefit from an educational design that stimulates the natural interplay between “intrinsic” (DMN) and “extrinsic” (CEN) brain networks by incorporating external stimulation (e.g., presenting content), allocating time and space for intrinsic reflection (e.g., guided reflection and journaling), and integrating the two (e.g., guided class discussion and insights sharing) [83][84].

References

  1. Thomas, M.S.C.; Ansari, D.; Knowland, V.C.P. Annual Research Review: Educational Neuroscience: Progress and Prospects. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2019, 60, 477–492.
  2. Ho, A.J.; Raji, C.A.; Becker, J.T.; Lopez, O.L.; Kuller, L.H.; Hua, X.; Dinov, I.D.; Stein, J.L.; Rosano, C.; Toga, A.W. The Effects of Physical Activity, Education, and Body Mass Index on the Aging Brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011, 32, 1371–1382.
  3. Berryhill, M.E.; Jones, K.T. TDCS Selectively Improves Working Memory in Older Adults with More Education. Neurosci. Lett. 2012, 521, 148–151.
  4. Schneeweis, N.; Skirbekk, V.; Winter-Ebmer, R. Does Education Improve Cognitive Performance Four Decades after School Completion? Demography 2014, 51, 619–643.
  5. Dahl, R.E. Adolescent Brain Development: A Period of Vulnerabilities and Opportunities. Keynote Address. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2004, 1021, 1–22.
  6. Lee, F.S.; Heimer, H.; Giedd, J.N.; Lein, E.S.; Šestan, N.; Weinberger, D.R.; Casey, B.J. Adolescent Mental Health—Opportunity and Obligation. Science 2014, 346, 547–549.
  7. Fuhrmann, D.; Knoll, L.J.; Blakemore, S.-J. Adolescence as a Sensitive Period of Brain Development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 558–566.
  8. Fuchs, E.; Flügge, G. Adult Neuroplasticity: More than 40 Years of Research. Neural Plast. 2014, 2014, 541870.
  9. Donati, G.; Meaburn, E. What Has Behavioural Genetic Research Told Us About the Origins of Individual Differences in Educational Abilities and Achievements? In Educational Neuroscience; Routledge: London, UK, 2020; pp. 53–87. ISBN 1003016839.
  10. Hackman, D.A.; Kraemer, D.J.M. Socioeconomic Disparities in Achievement: Insights on Neurocognitive Development and Educational Interventions. In Educational Neuroscience; Routledge: London, UK, 2020; pp. 88–119. ISBN 1003016839.
  11. Thomas, M.S.C.; Mareschal, D.; Dumontheil, I. Educational Neuroscience: Development across the Life Span; Routledge: London, UK, 2020; ISBN 1000040798.
  12. Weaver, I.C.G. Integrating Early Life Experience, Gene Expression, Brain Development, and Emergent Phenotypes: Unraveling the Thread of Nature via Nurture. Adv. Genet. 2014, 86, 277–307.
  13. Rangaswami, S. Nature, Nurture and the Learning Brain. In Neuro-Systemic Applications in Learning; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2021; pp. 333–368.
  14. Kass, J.; Jain, N. Neural Plasticity; Psychology Press Ltd.: London, UK, 2000; Volume 2, ISBN 0674007433.
  15. Mundkur N Neuroplasticity in Children. Indian J. Pediatr. 2005, 72, 855–857.
  16. Cramer, S.C.; Sur, M.; Dobkin, B.H.; O’Brien, C.; Sanger, T.D.; Trojanowski, J.Q.; Rumsey, J.M.; Hicks, R.; Cameron, J.; Chen, D. Harnessing Neuroplasticity for Clinical Applications. Brain 2011, 134, 1591–1609.
  17. Vygotsky, L.S. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1980; ISBN 0674076680.
  18. Davidson, R.J.; McEwen, B.S. Social Influences on Neuroplasticity: Stress and Interventions to Promote Well-Being. Nat. Neurosci. 2012, 15, 689–695.
  19. Voss, P.; Thomas, M.E.; Cisneros-Franco, J.M.; de Villers-Sidani, É. Dynamic Brains and the Changing Rules of Neuroplasticity: Implications for Learning and Recovery. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1657.
  20. Carey, L.M. Stroke Rehabilitation: Insights from Neuroscience and Imaging; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012; ISBN 0199797889.
  21. Kleim, J.A.; Jones, T.A. Principles of Experience-Dependent Neural Plasticity: Implications for Rehabilitation after Brain Damage. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2008, 51, S225–S239.
  22. Kolb, B.; Gibb, R. Brain Plasticity and Behaviour in the Developing Brain. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2011, 20, 265.
  23. Petrosini, L.; De Bartolo, P.; Foti, F.; Gelfo, F.; Cutuli, D.; Leggio, M.G.; Mandolesi, L. On Whether the Environmental Enrichment May Provide Cognitive and Brain Reserves. Brain Res. Rev. 2009, 61, 221–239.
  24. Dweck, C.S. Mindset: The New Psychology of Success; Random House Digital, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2008; ISBN 0345472322.
  25. Blackwell, L.S.; Trzesniewski, K.H.; Dweck, C.S. Implicit Theories of Intelligence Predict Achievement across an Adolescent Transition: A Longitudinal Study and an Intervention. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 246–263.
  26. Claro, S.; Paunesku, D.; Dweck, C.S. Growth Mindset Tempers the Effects of Poverty on Academic Achievement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 8664–8668.
  27. Broda, M.; Yun, J.; Schneider, B.; Yeager, D.S.; Walton, G.M.; Diemer, M. Reducing Inequality in Academic Success for Incoming College Students: A Randomized Trial of Growth Mindset and Belonging Interventions. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 2018, 11, 317–338.
  28. Stephens, N.M.; Hamedani, M.G.; Destin, M. Closing the Social-Class Achievement Gap: A Difference-Education Intervention Improves First-Generation Students’ Academic Performance and All Students’ College Transition. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 943–953.
  29. Walton, G.M.; Cohen, G.L. A Brief Social-Belonging Intervention Improves Academic and Health Outcomes of Minority Students. Science 2011, 331, 1447–1451.
  30. Zeng, G.; Hou, H.; Peng, K. Effect of Growth Mindset on School Engagement and Psychological Well-Being of Chinese Primary and Middle School Students: The Mediating Role of Resilience. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1873.
  31. Sisk, V.F.; Burgoyne, A.P.; Sun, J.; Butler, J.L.; Macnamara, B.N. To What Extent and under Which Circumstances Are Growth Mind-Sets Important to Academic Achievement? Two Meta-Analyses. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 549–571.
  32. Sarrasin, J.B.; Nenciovici, L.; Foisy, L.M.B.; Allaire-Duquette, G.; Riopel, M.; Masson, S. Effects of Teaching the Concept of Neuroplasticity to Induce a Growth Mindset on Motivation, Achievement, and Brain Activity: A Meta-Analysis. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2018, 12, 22–31.
  33. Austin, R.D.; Pisano, G.P. Neurodiversity as a Competitive Advantage. Harv. Bus. Rev. 2017, 95, 96–103.
  34. Huber, E.; Donnelly, P.M.; Rokem, A.; Yeatman, J.D. Rapid and Widespread White Matter Plasticity during an Intensive Reading Intervention. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 1–13.
  35. Romeo, R.R.; Christodoulou, J.A.; Halverson, K.K.; Murtagh, J.; Cyr, A.B.; Schimmel, C.; Chang, P.; Hook, P.E.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Socioeconomic Status and Reading Disability: Neuroanatomy and Plasticity in Response to Intervention. Cereb. Cortex 2018, 28, 2297–2312.
  36. Perdue, M.V.; Mahaffy, K.; Vlahcevic, K.; Wolfman, E.; Erbeli, F.; Richlan, F.; Landi, N. Reading Intervention and Neuroplasticity: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Brain Changes Associated with Reading Intervention. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 132, 465–494.
  37. McLaughlin, K.A.; Sheridan, M.A.; Lambert, H.K. Childhood Adversity and Neural Development: Deprivation and Threat as Distinct Dimensions of Early Experience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 47, 578–591.
  38. Bryck, R.L.; Fisher, P.A. Training the Brain: Practical Applications of Neural Plasticity from the Intersection of Cognitive Neuroscience, Developmental Psychology, and Prevention Science. Am. Psychol. 2012, 67, 87.
  39. Gapp, K.; Bohacek, J.; Grossmann, J.; Brunner, A.M.; Manuella, F.; Nanni, P.; Mansuy, I.M. Potential of Environmental Enrichment to Prevent Transgenerational Effects of Paternal Trauma. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016, 41, 2749–2758.
  40. Weist, M.D.; Eber, L.; Horner, R.; Splett, J.; Putnam, R.; Barrett, S.; Perales, K.; Fairchild, A.J.; Hoover, S. Improving Multitiered Systems of Support for Students With “Internalizing” Emotional/Behavioral Problems. J. Posit. Behav. Interv. 2018, 20, 172–184.
  41. Arias-Carrión, O.; Stamelou, M.; Murillo-Rodríguez, E.; Menéndez-González, M.; Pöppel, E. Dopaminergic Reward System: A Short Integrative Review. Int. Arch. Med. 2010, 3, 1–6.
  42. Berridge, K.C.; Robinson, T.E.; Aldridge, J.W. Dissecting Components of Reward: “Liking”, “Wanting”, and Learning. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 2009, 9, 65–73.
  43. Ormrod, J.E. Human Learning; Merrill Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1999; ISBN 0138756848.
  44. Wong, R. When No One Can Go to School: Does Online Learning Meet Students’ Basic Learning Needs? Interact. Learn. Environ. 2020, 1–17.
  45. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic Motivation, Social Development, and Well-Being. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 68–78.
  46. Bandura, A.; Freeman, W.H.; Lightsey, R. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control; Worth Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 1999.
  47. Krapp, A. Basic Needs and the Development of Interest and Intrinsic Motivational Orientations. Learn. Instr. 2005, 15, 381–395.
  48. Berridge, K.C. Evolving Concepts of Emotion and Motivation. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1–20.
  49. Degenhardt, L.; Stockings, E.; Patton, G.; Hall, W.D.; Lynskey, M. The Increasing Global Health Priority of Substance Use in Young People. Lancet Psychiatry 2016, 3, 251–264.
  50. Poudel, A.; Gautam, S. Age of Onset of Substance Use and Psychosocial Problems among Individuals with Substance Use Disorders. BMC Psychiatry 2017, 17, 1–7.
  51. Trujillo, C.A.; Obando, D.; Trujillo, A. An Examination of the Association between Early Initiation of Substance Use and Interrelated Multilevel Risk and Protective Factors among Adolescents. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0225384.
  52. Immordino-yang, M.H. Embodied Brains, Social Minds, Cultural Meaning: Integrating Neuroscientific and Educational Research on Social-Affective Development. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2017, 54, 344–367.
  53. Jennings, P.A.; Greenberg, M.T. The Prosocial Classroom: Teacher Social and Emotional Competence in Relation to Student and Classroom Outcomes. Rev. Educ. Res. 2009, 79, 491–525.
  54. Okonofua, J.A.; Paunesku, D.; Walton, G.M. Brief Intervention to Encourage Empathic Discipline Cuts Suspension Rates in Half among Adolescents. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 5221–5226.
  55. Battro, A.M.; Fischer, K.W.; Léna, P.J. Introduction: Mind, Brain, and Education in Theory and Practice. Educ. Brain Essays Neuroeducation 2008, 1–19.
  56. Immordino-Yang, M.H.; Darling-Hammond, L.; Krone, C.R. Nurturing Nature: How Brain Development Is Inherently Social and Emotional, and What This Means for Education. Educ. Psychol. 2019, 54, 185–204.
  57. Lieberman, M.D. Social Cognitive Neuroscience. Encycl. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 143–193.
  58. Allen, V.L.; Feldman, R.S. Learning through Tutoring: Low-Achieving Children as Tutors. J. Exp. Educ. 1973, 42, 1–5.
  59. Rohrbeck, C.A.; Ginsburg-Block, M.D.; Fantuzzo, J.W.; Miller, T.R. Peer-Assisted Learning Interventions with Elementary School Students: A Meta-Analytic Review. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, 240.
  60. Topping, K.J. The Effectiveness of Peer Tutoring in Further and Higher Education: A Typology and Review of the Literature. High. Educ. 1996, 32, 321–345.
  61. Mehrabian, A. Silent Messages; Wadsworth: Belmont, CA, USA, 1971; Volume 8, ISBN 0534000592.
  62. Richmond, V.P.; Gorham, J.S.; McCroskey, J.C. The Relationship between Selected Immediacy Behaviors and Cognitive Learning. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 1987, 10, 574–590.
  63. Christophel, D.M. The Relationships among Teacher Immediacy Behaviors, Student Motivation, and Learning. Commun. Educ. 1990, 39, 323–340.
  64. Kelley, D.H.; Gorham, J. Effects of Immediacy on Recall of Information. Commun. Educ. 1988, 37, 198–207.
  65. Liu, W. Does Teacher Immediacy Affect Students? A Systematic Review of the Association between Teacher Verbal and Non-Verbal Immediacy and Student Motivation. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 2475.
  66. Frymier, A.B.; Shulman, G.M. “What’s in It for Me?”: Increasing Content Relevance to Enhance Students’ Motivation. Commun. Educ. 1995, 44, 40–50.
  67. Stevens, C.; Bavelier, D. The Role of Selective Attention on Academic Foundations: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012, 2 (Suppl. 1), S30–S48.
  68. Markett, S.; Nothdurfter, D.; Focsa, A.; Reuter, M.; Jawinski, P. Attention Networks and the Intrinsic Network Structure of the Human Brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2022, 43, 1431–1448.
  69. Geva, R.; Zivan, M.; Warsha, A.; Olchik, D. Alerting, Orienting or Executive Attention Networks: Differential Patters of Pupil Dilations. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 145.
  70. Vossel, S.; Geng, J.J.; Fink, G.R. Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems: Distinct Neural Circuits but Collaborative Roles. Neuroscientist 2014, 20, 150–159.
  71. Corbetta, M.; Shulman, G.L. Control of Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Driven Attention in the Brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 201–215.
  72. Menon, V.; Uddin, L.Q. Saliency, Switching, Attention and Control: A Network Model of Insula Function. Brain Struct. Funct. 2010, 214, 655–667.
  73. Onofrj, V.; Chiarelli, A.M.; Wise, R.; Colosimo, C.; Caulo, M. Interaction of the Salience Network, Ventral Attention Network, Dorsal Attention Network and Default Mode Network in Neonates and Early Development of the Bottom-up Attention System. Brain Struct. Funct. 2022, 227, 1843–1856.
  74. Sridharan, D.; Levitin, D.J.; Menon, V. A Critical Role for the Right Fronto-Insular Cortex in Switching between Central-Executive and Default-Mode Networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 12569–12574.
  75. Immordino-Yang, M.H.; Darling-Hammond, L.; Krone, C. The Brain Basis for Integrated Social, Emotional, and Academic Development: How Emotions and Social Relationships Drive Learning. Natl. Comm. Soc. Emot. Acad. Dev. 2018, 20.
  76. Lieberman, M.D. Education and the Social Brain. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2012, 1, 3–9.
  77. Chen, A.C.; Oathes, D.J.; Chang, C.; Bradley, T.; Zhou, Z.-W.; Williams, L.M.; Glover, G.H.; Deisseroth, K.; Etkin, A. Causal Interactions between Fronto-Parietal Central Executive and Default-Mode Networks in Humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 19944–19949.
  78. D’esposito, M.; Detre, J.A.; Alsop, D.C.; Shin, R.K.; Atlas, S.; Grossman, M. The Neural Basis of the Central Executive System of Working Memory. Nature 1995, 378, 279–281.
  79. Zhou, Y.; Friston, K.J.; Zeidman, P.; Chen, J.; Li, S.; Razi, A. The Hierarchical Organization of the Default, Dorsal Attention and Salience Networks in Adolescents and Young Adults. Cereb. Cortex 2018, 28, 726–737.
  80. Uddin, L.Q.; Clare Kelly, A.M.; Biswal, B.B.; Xavier Castellanos, F.; Milham, M.P. Functional Connectivity of Default Mode Network Components: Correlation, Anticorrelation, and Causality. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 625–637.
  81. Reineberg, A.E.; Gustavson, D.E.; Benca, C.; Banich, M.T.; Friedman, N.P. The Relationship between Resting State Network Connectivity and Individual Differences in Executive Functions. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1600.
  82. Manoliu, A.; Meng, C.; Brandl, F.; Doll, A.; Tahmasian, M.; Scherr, M.; Schwerthöffer, D.; Zimmer, C.; Förstl, H.; Bäuml, J. Insular Dysfunction within the Salience Network Is Associated with Severity of Symptoms and Aberrant Inter-Network Connectivity in Major Depressive Disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 7, 930.
  83. Yang, X.F.; Pavarini, G.; Schnall, S.; Immordino-Yang, M.H. Looking up to Virtue: Averting Gaze Facilitates Moral Construals via Posteromedial Activations. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2018, 13, 1131–1139.
  84. Immordino-yang, M.H.; Knecht, D.R. Building Meaning Builds Teens’ Brains. Educ. Leadersh. 2020, 77, 36–43.
More
Information
Contributors MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register : ,
View Times: 357
Revisions: 2 times (View History)
Update Date: 29 Jan 2023
1000/1000