Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 + 1573 word(s) 1573 2022-01-27 07:03:28

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Muffato, V. Orientation Experiences and Navigation Aid Use in Lifespan. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/18904 (accessed on 04 August 2024).
Muffato V. Orientation Experiences and Navigation Aid Use in Lifespan. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/18904. Accessed August 04, 2024.
Muffato, Veronica. "Orientation Experiences and Navigation Aid Use in Lifespan" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/18904 (accessed August 04, 2024).
Muffato, V. (2022, January 27). Orientation Experiences and Navigation Aid Use in Lifespan. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/18904
Muffato, Veronica. "Orientation Experiences and Navigation Aid Use in Lifespan." Encyclopedia. Web. 27 January, 2022.
Orientation Experiences and Navigation Aid Use in Lifespan
Edit

Spatial orientation is essential for daily life, but it deteriorates with aging. The present study was aimed at investigating age changes across the adult lifespan in the self-reported use of navigation aids (maps, GPS, and verbal directions) and everyday orientation experiences (how much they went out, and how much they reached or lost their way to unfamiliar destinations). We also investigated to what extent these spatial behaviours are related to people’s visuospatial working memory (VSWM) and self-reported wayfinding inclinations.

spatial behaviors visuospatial working memory sense of direction spatial anxiety GPS map aging navigation lifespan

1. Introduction

The abilities to navigate and orient oneself to the environment are essential in daily life for one to successfully reach destinations and avoid getting lost. Such abilities have been shown to decline with increasing age [1]. These impairments can limit older people’s independence and safety of living, thus having an impact on their perceived quality of life [2]. Therefore, within the healthy and active aging domain perspective, it is important to extend our theoretical knowledge on older people’s navigation issues and on the factors that can be related to them.
Among spatial behaviors across the adult lifespan, it is of interest to examine the everyday orientation experiences (going out, reaching places, and/or experiences of getting lost) [3] and the use of navigation aids. They comprise the instruments (e.g., maps, global positioning system-GPS) and modalities (e.g., asking for verbal directions) used to support moving in the environment. People's spatial behaviours are of interest for the identification of people’s spatial profiles and, related to this, for people’s navigation performance accuracy and navigation habits. Research has that aging changes people’s spatial behaviors, such as their mobility, experiences of reaching places or getting lost, and the use of navigation aids. Research showed a decrease in map learning with aging and learning a route from a map before did not consequently help with the path navigation process. Also, having a map available while navigating did not increase the navigation performance[4]. However, older adults felt more secure when the map was there. Concerning GPS use, it seems to positively support navigation. When older adults are trained to use GPS tools, they increase their successful driving (e.g., reaching the destination). Furthermore, older adults reported using GPS to plan their trips to minimize their self-reported fear of getting lost[5]. On the contrary, spatial verbal directions appeared not to support spatial mental representations of older adults compared with other inputs, such as maps[6]. However, such navigation-based spatial behaviors could relate to individual factors other than age, such as people spatial abilities and wayfinding inclinations.

2. Rationale and Aims

The present study started from the premise that self-reported spatial behaviors could be informative of people’s navigation difficulties related to their objective spatial abilities. The literature showed age changes namely increasing age from youth to older age in self-reported spatial behaviors Apart from age, an adequate spatial profile [7] in terms of spatial abilities (VSWM) and inclinations could support functional spatial behaviors. However, little evidence regarding the young has suggested a relation between self-reported spatial behaviors in everyday life (use of navigation aids) and individual visuospatial factors . This issue merits being investigated with an adult lifespan perspective to better capture any age-related changes. In fact, in the adult lifespan, evidence exists that environment-based self-reports are linked, even if differently, to spatial performance, and individual visuospatial factors are related to environmental performance[8]. It is plausible that self-reported spatial behaviors (people-orientation experiences and navigation aid use) might also be linked to individual visuospatial factors. This issue has yet to be investigated. Examining age, visuospatial abilities, and people’s attitudes in their relationship to spatial behavior contribute to a better understanding of older adults’ everyday mobility. The self-reported experience could be informative about the impact of navigation issues on the daily life of older adults, to promote healthy and active aging. In this context, the present study was aimed at investigating from an adult lifespan perspective the relationship among self-reported everyday spatial behaviors, such as orientation experiences (tendency to go out, successfully reaching an unfamiliar destination, or getting lost), people’s use of navigation aids (maps, GPS, verbal directions), individual visuospatial factors in terms of VSWM, and one’s wayfinding inclinations (sense of direction, pleasure in exploring places, and spatial anxiety).

3. Methods

A sample of 456 people aged 25-84 years rated how much they use navigation aids (maps, GPS, verbal directions), how much they went out, and how much they reached or lost their way to unfamiliar destinations[7]. Then, they performed the jigsaw puzzle test (VSWM) and questionnaires on sense of direction, pleasure in exploring, and spatial anxiety [9][10].

4. Results

Linear models were run stepwise to determine whether the factors added to each step improved the model.

Everyday orientation experiences

For going out, Step 0 accounted for 4% of the variance, with both gender and years of education emerging as a significant predictor. Age (Step 1) emerged as a significant predictor and accounted for another 4% of the variance. JPT (VSWM, Step 2) emerged as a significant predictor, meaning that higher JPT scores are associated with a higher frequency of going out, accounting for 2% of the variance. Step 3 accounted for 3% of the variance, with spatial anxiety emerging as a significant predictor, meaning that a higher level of anxiety is associated with a lower frequency of going out.

For the experiences of reaching an unfamiliar place (total R2 = 0.12), Step 0 accounted for 2% of the variance, with years of education emerging as a significant predictor. Age (Step 1) emerged as a significant predictor and accounted for 2% of the variance. JPT (VSWM, step 2) emerged as a significant predictor, meaning that higher JPT scores is associated with a higher frequency of successfully reaching unfamiliar places, accounting for 4% of the variance. Step 3 accounted for another 4% of the variance, with spatial anxiety emerging as a significant predictor, meaning that a higher level of anxiety reduces the frequency of reaching an unfamiliar place.

For the experiences of losing one’s way in an unfamiliar place (total R2 = 0.22), Step 0 represented 4% of the variance, with years of education emerging as a significant predictor. Age (Step 1) emerged as a significant predictor and accounted for 8% of the variance. JPT (VSWM, Step 2) did not add any variance. Step 3 accounted for another 10% of the variance, with SDSR and spatial anxiety emerging as significant predictors, meaning that a low level of anxiety and a high sense of direction reduces the frequency of getting lost in an unfamiliar place.

Everyday navigation aids use.

For the use of the map (total R2 = 0.17), Step 0 accounted for 10% of the variance, and years of education emerged as a significant predictor. Age (Step 1) did not emerge as a significant predictor and accounted for 1% of the variance. JPT (VSWM, Step 2) emerged as significant predictor, meaning that higher JPT scores are associated with the increased use of maps, accounting for 3% of the variance. Step 3 accounted for another 3% of the variance, with AtOT and spatial anxiety emerging as significant predictors, meaning that greater attitudes toward orientation and higher spatial anxiety are associated with higher map use.

For GPS use (total R2 = 0.35), Step 0 accounted for 13% of the variance, with both gender and years of education emerging as significant predictors. Age (Step 1) emerged as a significant predictor and accounted for 19% of the variance. JPT (VSWM, Step 2) emerged as a significant predictor, meaning that higher JPT scores were associated with the increased use of GPS, accounting for 3% of the variance. Step 3 did not account for any variance.

For the use of verbal instructions (total R2 = 0.09), Step 0 accounted for 3% of the variance, with gender emerging as a significant predictor. Age (Step 1) emerged as a significant predictor and accounted for 3% of the variance. JPT (VSWM, step 2) emerged as a significant predictor, meaning that high JPT scores were associated with a lower use of verbal instructions, accounting for another 3% of the variance. Step 3 did not account for any additional variance.

4. Discussion and conclusion

The present study shed light on people’s self-assessed spatial behaviors in relation to individual differences, adopting an adult lifespan perspective. Considering the role of increasing age, associated with unsuccessful spatial behaviors, the role of spatial abilities (VSWM) and positive wayfinding inclinations played a role in promoting spatial behaviors. On the other hand, the role of spatial anxiety is detrimental for spatial behaviors, especially for losing in an unfamiliar place. For promoting active and healthy aging with regard to maintaining people’s mobility and active use of navigation aids, it seems to be important to consider people spatial competences and wayfinding inclinations from youth to older age.

Taking into account the limitations and the results of the present study, future directions could be focused on efforts to promote healthy and active aging. Several individual factors relating to their spatial behaviors should be considered and thoroughly investigated in future studies to support older people’s mobility and interest in exploring the environment. For example, their use of GPS for navigation should be analyzed in depth, given our increased reliance on GPS, even in aging [11], and the mixed findings on its influence on young and older adults’ navigation accuracy [5]. Regarding the various individual differences relating to successful spatial behaviors, future studies could examine how to improve older adults’ everyday spatial behaviors, by giving them training on the efficient use of orientation strategies (such as maps and GPS), basic cognitive abilities (such as VSWM), and on metacognitive reflection of their own wayfinding inclinations, for instance.

This entry is adapted from doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031225

References

  1. Ineke J.M. Van Der Ham; Michiel H.G. Claessen; How age relates to spatial navigation performance: Functional and methodological considerations. Ageing Research Reviews 2020, 58, 101020, 10.1016/j.arr.2020.101020.
  2. I Ham; N Kant; A Postma; J Visser-Meily; Is navigation ability a problem in mild stroke patients? Insights from self-reported navigation measures. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 2013, 45, 429-433, 10.2340/16501977-1139.
  3. Peter C. Burns; Navigation and the Mobility of Older Drivers. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 1999, 54, S49-S55, 10.1093/geronb/54b.1.s49.
  4. Marie Sjölinder; Kristina Höök; Lars-Göran Nilsson; Gerd Andersson; Age differences and the acquisition of spatial knowledge in a three-dimensional environment: Evaluating the use of an overview map as a navigation aid. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 2005, 63, 537-564, 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2005.04.024.
  5. Anne E. Dickerson; Use of GPS for Older Adults to Decrease Driving Risk: Perceptions from Users and Non-Users. Geriatrics 2020, 5, 60, 10.3390/geriatrics5030060.
  6. Chiara Meneghetti; Erika Borella; Irene Grasso; Rossana De Beni; Learning a route using a map and/or description in young and older adults. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 2012, 24, 165-178, 10.1080/20445911.2011.603694.
  7. Chiara Meneghetti; Francesco Grimaldi; Massimo Nucci; Francesca Pazzaglia; Positive and Negative Wayfinding Inclinations, Choice of Navigation Aids, and How They Relate to Personality Traits. Journal of Individual Differences 2020, 41, 45-52, 10.1027/1614-0001/a000302.
  8. Chiara Meneghetti; Laura Miola; Enrico Toffalini; Massimiliano Pastore; Francesca Pazzaglia; Learning from navigation, and tasks assessing its accuracy: The role of visuospatial abilities and wayfinding inclinations. Journal of Environmental Psychology 2021, 75, 101614, 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101614.
  9. Veronica Muffato; Chiara Meneghetti; Rossana De Beni; Spatial mental representations: the influence of age on route learning from maps and navigation. Psychological Research 2018, 83, 1836-1850, 10.1007/s00426-018-1033-4.
  10. Veronica Muffato; Rossana De Beni; Path Learning From Navigation in Aging: The Role of Cognitive Functioning and Wayfinding Inclinations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2020, 14, 6, 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00008.
  11. Masashi Sugimoto; Takashi Kusumi; Noriko Nagata; Toru Ishikawa; Online mobile map effect: how smartphone map use impairs spatial memory. Spatial Cognition & Computation 2021, 29, 1-23, 10.1080/13875868.2021.1969401.
More
Information
Subjects: Psychology
Contributor MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register :
View Times: 472
Revision: 1 time (View History)
Update Date: 27 Jan 2022
1000/1000
Video Production Service