Radial Arm Maze Task: History
Please note this is an old version of this entry, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

The Radial Arm Maze (RAM), developed by Olton and Samuelson (1976) and quickly adapted in humans, is a high ecological spatial task, firstly used in a real environment and subsequently in the virtual one.

  • human navigation
  • virtual reality
  • behavioral task
  • spatial abilities
  • large-scale task

1. Radial Arm Maze Task (RAM)

1.1. Free-Choice and Forced-Choice Version

The Radial Arm Maze (RAM) task, developed by Olton and Samuelson (1976) to assess the spatial abilities in rodents, is also used in several studies on children [1][2][3][4][5][6] and adults [7][8][9].
RAM consists of a central holding area from which several identical arms, commonly eight, radiate, and the task’s difficulty depends on the number of them. There is a hidden reward at the end of each arm, generally a coin or a little toy for children.
Different RAM paradigms take into account the environmental cues and the kind of spatial memory process to be investigated. Generally, it is possible to distinguish two main classic paradigms: free-choice and forced-choice RAM versions (Figure 1).
/media/item_content/202205/6281b480ebcacbrainsci-12-00468-g002.png
Figure 1. Free-choice (a) and forced-choice (b) paradigms are represented.
In the free-choice version, the subjects have to take all the rewards and know that the arms are rewarded only once (declarative rule). To solve the task without errors, the subject has to make use of mnesic and mapping abilities, as well as proficient explorative strategies [3][10][11]. Carrying out the RAM in several trials, these competencies can be learned during the various phases of the task.
For many years, the free-choice RAM version has been considered appropriate for evaluating the correct functioning of working-term memory abilities by detecting the number of errors (e.g., returning to arms already visited). However, some authors observed that the longest sequence of correctly visited arms, corresponding to spatial span parameter, can also depend on the type of strategy put into action to explore the maze, suggesting the potential that RAM offers to evaluate procedural memory processes [12][13]. In addition, employing different parameters, the free-choice RAM version allows to efficaciously study the explorative strategies used by the subject. For example, it is possible to analyze if he/she visits a specific sequence of arms or always beginning a run from the same arm (“praxic” strategy), or if he/she solves the task by referring to specific environmental stimuli (“taxic” strategy), or finally, if he/she exploits mapping abilities to build a cognitive spatial map (“place” strategy) [14][15][16]. To refer to these different strategies, numerous terms, sich as “motor”, “cue”, or “relational” strategies, respectively, have been used in other studies [3][15].
Therefore, to distinguish explorative from working mnesic components, it is possible to use the forced-choice RAM version. In this protocol, each trial consists of two phases. In the first phase, the subjects have to collect only four rewards, while the remaining ones are inaccessible. In the second phase, he/she has to collect the rewards of the four arms not visited in the first phase. Success depends on remembering the arms visited in the first phase (rather than putting into action particular search patterns), thus emphasizing working memory requirements. Putting into action a specific exploratory strategy is avoided using different angles to separate the opened arms (i.e., arms 2,4,5,8). Therefore, although both paradigms investigate spatial memory processes, they analyze different aspects of these processes.
Both versions of RAM can be cued or uncued [3][17]. In the cued RAM version, each arm is made physically distinct by visual stimulus at its end. In the uncued RAM version, visited arms can be remembered by the subjects in relation to their spatial relationship to distal extra maze cues. It is easy to understand that when using the cued RAM version, the subject is forced to apply a taxic strategy to solve the task, while an allocentric strategy in the uncued RAM version is more appropriate. Again, the type of paradigm allows to investigate different aspects of spatial exploration.
Therefore, the choice of a specific RAM paradigm depends on the type of study objective to be achieved, and the normotypical and clinical population to be studied. For example, for children around the age of four years, who have not yet developed short-term memory processes, the use of free choice may be more appropriate.
In Table 1, the main parameters analyzed in free-choice and forced-choice RAM versions are reported to emphasize the different faces of memory components that can be studied throughout real and virtual RAM tasks.
Table 1. Illustration of the main parameters used to analyze the performances in RAM task.

Free-Choice RAM Version

Forced-Choice RAM Version

(Referred to the Second Phase of the Task)

Total time to complete the entire task

Time to reach each reward

Total time to complete the second phase of the task

Latency to select the first arm

Latency to select the first arm

Total entries (arms correct and incorrect visited)

Total entries (arms correct and incorrect visited)

Distance travelled

Distance travelled

Movement speed

Errors

Frequency of successes/Percentage of correct visits/Search efficiency

Across-phase errors

Errors/Error-free trials

Within-phase errors

The longest sequence of correctly visited arms

The longest sequence of correctly visited arms

Percentage of angles turned (45°, 90°, 135°, 180° or 360°)/Angle change/Strategy fixation

 

Perseverations (consecutive entries into the same arm or the re-entries into a fixed sequence of arms)

 

Declarative mastery

 
It is appropriate to note that in free-choice and forced-choice RAM versions, the subject walks around the maze, and this promotes the integration of the mechanisms that link perception to action. This feature suggests that RAM is a complete task as it allows to also analyze perceptive and motor processes. Furthermore, the exploration of an environment through moving in it accelerates the spatial learning processes, allowing the formation of a spatial cognitive map[18], thus indicating RAM as a tool for devising virtual personalized neurorehabilitation training, as is already being done with other experimental protocols [19].

1.2. Table RAM and Visuospatial Peripersonal Abilities

The RAM is cataloged among large-scale behavioral tasks since it is a walking task. The subjects are inside the maze and see it from the inside, thus promoting an allocentric and egocentric encoding. The participant is compelled to build a spatial cognitive map of RAM to orient and move himself/herself in it. In this way, the declarative competence of the environment is probably built through procedural competence [13]. Recently, Foti and collaborators have developed a RAM table version that allows studying the visuospatial peripersonal abilities through body–objects interaction [20]. In fact, in this table RAM version, the participant is forced to explore the portion of space accessible with the limbs in order to resolve the task, which was presented to children as the “Ladybug game”. The child had to move the older sister ladybug, placed on the central platform, to find its sisters hidden inside the caps at the end of each arm [20]. The child is seated in front of the RAM and has visual access to the maze in all its completeness. Seeing it from above, it is likely that the construction of the spatial cognitive map may be facilitated because declarative knowledge is promptly formed. In addition, recent scientific literature reported that tactile and visual stimuli inside the peripersonal space elicit stronger processing and induce a powerful multilevel activation [21], inducing an integration of perceptive, motor, and cognitive processes. When we see an object and recognize its function, we also know how to grasp it, preparing ourselves for the action to be enacted upon it. These characteristics related to the process that links the perception to the action suggest the table RAM is an advantageous tool to improve peripersonal spatial abilities. Furthermore, in this RAM table version, the two RAM paradigms, free choice and forced choice, were administered. However, this time it is necessary to point out that the free choice paradigm served as habituation to the setting. In contrast, the forced choice paradigm constituted the experimental part of the study. The reason for this is easy to understand, as on a small scale, free exploration is elementary, even for children. In the future, it may be helpful to administer free choice to populations with marked cognitive deficits, such as in neglect syndrome.
In this line of thinking, it is interesting to note that another group of researchers has used a small-scale RAM model to investigate the age at which children begin to integrate the increasing flexibility in the conjoint use of egocentric and allocentric frames of reference [22], obtaining data comparable to those of classic neuropsychological spatial tests, such as Corsi Block task or block construction [1][14], indicating also the reliability of this ecological task. In the past, O’Connor and Glassman used a radial maze analog drawn on paper to study short-term memory[7], first suggesting the RAM as a tabletop tool.

2. Applications of RAM Task in Real Environment

As described above, RAM is a behavioral ecological task on a large and small scale that allows the analyses of different facets of spatial memory. In humans, several clinical and psychological studies have extensively used the walking RAM version for analyzing the navigational abilities in individuals with typical development (TD) and the spatial deficit in specific clinical populations.
In the late ‘80s, walking RAM was used in children to study spatial memory and understand from what age it could be administered[2][4]. These studies have shown that even preschool infants can walk in RAM. However, the variable dimensions regarding length and number of arms and the experimental setting have confused the results. About ten years after these pioneering studies, Overmann and colleagues developed a RAM built to human scale in which children were tested without explicit verbal instructions and with a longitudinal procedure for up to 16 consecutive weekdays, using free-choice and forced-choice versions[3]. In a sense, Overmann’s study confirms the precedents, even though it aimed to observe the development of mapping abilities rather than evaluate the age of administration of RAM. Successively, other behavioral studies on TD children were carried out employing both versions of the walking RAM to investigate the ontogenesis of spatial competencies and eventually gender differences [1][23][24][6] as well, so as to better characterize the spatial deficit in adolescents with Williams and Prader-Willi syndromes [25][14] and to evaluate the spatial orientation of intrauterine growth retarded children[5]. All these studies have shown how RAM can analyze the development of a process (spatial abilities) and highlight the presence and severity of a spatial deficit.
Recently, the walking RAM task has also been used to compare learning by observation to learning by doing in TD children [26]. In this study, the authors have made clear that the observation of the correct explorative strategy showed by the experimenter promotes the development of spatial declarative and procedural knowledge, thus suggesting the RAM task as a useful tool for improving and facilitating spatial memory. In particular, the authors highlighted that the observation of a correct exploration strategy, such as the entry into the adjacent arms, induces an early development of the spatial cognitive map in the observing child. This study suggests that RAM can also be an educational tool to facilitate and accelerate learning processes.
Even in adults, the first studies that used the walking RAM date back to the 1980s. In some of these, participants’ performances were compared to those of the rats in analog mazes [7]. Successively, the RAM task was mainly used to study human navigation behavior in health and clinical populations [7][8][9][6][17][27], highlighting once again how RAM can be used for diagnostic purposes. Recently, a RAM version has been also used to evidence physical activity effects on spatial abilities [28]. In fact, by comparing the performance of athletes with those of a sedentary group, it was possible to highlight how physical exercise improves spatial memory.
However, in these studies, the behavioral procedure is not always comparable. For example, in some of them, it is preferred to use the free choice version with only part of the arms baited [29], or to insert specific cue intramaze, or change the starting arm [17]. As already pointed out, the choice of one or the other version of the RAM task depends on the age of the participants and on the type of memory process to be studied.
Although these differences make the results confusing and not homogeneous, they demonstrate once again the extent to which RAM task is a flexible tool that can be easily adapted to the type of spatial process to be investigated and the type of deficit to be rehabilitated.

3. Potentiality and Applications of RAM Task in Virtual Environment

The RAM task is a highly ecological test because it is administered outside hospital environments and experimental settings of research laboratories. Aside for a few examples, RAM is a large-scale task that is presented as a game, especially in children. When considering the different RAM paradigms and versions, overall, on the one hand, they have favored objectives and reliable results, also correlating to aseptic paper and pencil tests. However, on the other hand, their design has hampered RAM use, as it is very expensive to assemble them in real environments. Furthermore, as RAM tasks are very often performed outdoors and generally last a few days, they are also affected by weather conditions. All these difficulties may explain why the RAM task is only partially used in humans compared to its extensive application in animal research and the numerous evidences in the implementation in virtual modality made possible by VR technology progress. As has been already pointed out, VR offers several advantages, such as the possibility to evaluate people in complete safety [30][31]. Another possible advantage consists of manipulating the environment, for example, making it increasingly complex or easier to explore, thus allowing for more personalization as well as a more interactive subject-environment. In addition, the changes that can be made in virtual modality allow to specifically investigate the type of strategy used by the participant to solve the task. While in real RAM version, for example, it is not certain whether the participant has oriented himself/herself according to the external cues, which, although kept under control cannot be stable (for example, a strong wind, variable brightness, etc.). In virtual RAM version, it is possible to modify the surrounding landmarks and keep other conditions constant, analyzing the procedural competences in more detail. Despite this, most of the studies that have used RAM in virtual modality have adopted the forced-choice version of the task, which allows analyzing working and short-term memory processes rather than the type of strategy used by the subject. A possible explanation could be that the free choice version is apparently easier than the forced-choice one, and since the participants were mainly young adults, the researchers believed it more useful to administer a RAM version emphasizing working memory requirements.
Other potential advantages relate to the fact that the digitized versions of the RAM task can be easily shared by several groups of researchers, and that the data obtained can be entered into scientific databases. With the aim of eventually implementing rehabilitative intervention, it could be possible to imagine a sort of “videogame training” that the patient can perform inside his/her home when motivated to do so.
To the researchers' knowledge, the first evidence of a virtual RAM task goes back to the Iaria et al. study in 2003. The authors created an eight-arm radial maze with a central starting location. The maze was surrounded by a landscape (mountains and sunset), two trees, and a short wall located between the landscape and the tree. At the end of each arm, there was a staircase leading to the location where an object could be picked up in some of the arms. The participants were young, healthy adults who used a keypad to move in any direction [32]. Successively, joysticks were also used to navigate through the virtual RAM, but the subjects were always seated in front of the computer [33][34][35][36][29][37][10][38][11]. In 2012, a study of 599, including TD children and younger to older healthy adults, demonstrated the virtual RAM task to be a useful tool with which to investigate the changes in exploratory strategies over life span [39], confirming the results of the studies conducted with real RAM, but adding valuable information as to environmental factors that can modulate the development of navigational strategies. In the last twenty years, studies with virtual RAM tasks have greatly increased, and more and more evidence also relates to clinical populations and children, as well as the suggestion of new paradigms of task-based RAM [40][41][42][43][44][45][46][47][48][36][49][29]. For example, Marsh and colleagues (2015) had administered a virtual eight RAM version during fMRI scanning of adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) in order to study the functioning of mesolimbic and striatal areas involved in reward-based spatial learning [44]. Furthermore, other authors investigated navigational strategies in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) children [43]. The use of virtual RAM in these clinical populations suggests how it is suitable for individuals exhibiting behavioral alterations.
Recently, a digitalized version of the RAM task was also used to investigate the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the spatial exploration in Italian University students, allowing to evidence an increase in pseudoneglect through analysis of the lateralization of the first explored arm [50]. However, many studies use RAM in non-immersive modality to evaluate spatial abilities [51][52][53][54][55][56][57][58][59][60][61][62][63].
To date, only two studies reported the virtual RAM task in full immersive modality [64][65]. In particular, Kim et al. have developed a virtual RAM task with a head-mounted display to produce information about travel distance and head movement, demonstrating that this virtual task was just as competent as the walking task one in measuring spatial learning and memory [64]. More recently, Ben-Zeev and colleagues have produced a virtual RAM task in which the subjects wore specific virtual reality goggles as a display that enabled them to see the room in a first-person perspective, as well as a rotating tool of the view, due to its capability to translate head movements in real-time as shifts of the viewpoint [65].

From the analysis of the studies carried out, it is clear that most of them use the forced choice paradigm, and this observation deserves careful consideration. Once again, the two paradigms allowed to evaluate different facets of spatial memory. Still, the forced choice method is more sensitive to the short memory components, and is also more challenging to perform. However, in virtual modality, it is easier to modify scenarios by reducing (or increasing) the complexity of the task. Perhaps this could be why forced choice in virtual modality is more frequent than free choice.

This entry is adapted from the peer-reviewed paper 10.3390/brainsci12040468

References

  1. L. Mandolesi; L. Petrosini; D. Menghini; F. Addona; S. Vicari; Children's radial arm maze performance as a function of age and sex. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 2009, 27, 789-797, 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2009.08.010.
  2. Nigel P. Foreman; Margaret Arber; Joe Savage; Spatial memory in preschool infants. Developmental Psychobiology 1984, 17, 129-137, 10.1002/dev.420170204.
  3. W. H. Overman; Bobbi Jean Pate; Kim Moore; Andrea Peuster; Ontogeny of place learning in children as measured in the Radial Arm Maze, Morris Search Task, and Open Field Task.. Behavioral Neuroscience 1996, 110, 1205-1228, 10.1037//0735-7044.110.6.1205.
  4. Jan Aadland; William W. Beatty; Ruth H. Maki; Spatial memory of children and adults assessed in the radial maze. Developmental Psychobiology 1985, 18, 163-172, 10.1002/dev.420180208.
  5. Y. Leitner; D. Heldman; S. Harel; C.G. Pick; Deficits in spatial orientation of children with intrauterine growth retardation. Brain Research Bulletin 2005, 67, 13-18, 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.04.017.
  6. Léa Bertholet; Manuel Torres Escobar; Marion Depré; Camille F. Chavan; Fabienne Giuliani; Pascale Gisquet-Verrier; Delphine Preissmann; Françoise Schenk; Spatial radial maze procedures and setups to dissociate local and distal relational spatial frameworks in humans. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 2015, 253, 126-141, 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.06.012.
  7. R.Corey O'Connor; Robert B. Glassman; Human performance with a seventeen-arm radial maze analog. Brain Research Bulletin 1993, 30, 189-191, 10.1016/0361-9230(93)90058-j.
  8. Robert B. Glassman; Kimberly J. Garvey; Kimberly M. Elkins; Kimberly L. Kasal; Nicole L. Couillard; Spatial working memory score of humans in a large radial maze, similar to published score of rats, implies capacity close to the magical number 7 ± 2. Brain Research Bulletin 1994, 34, 151-159, 10.1016/0361-9230(94)90012-4.
  9. Robert B. Glassman; Kimberly M. Leniek; Tamara M. Haegerich; Human working memory capacity is 7 ± 2 in a radial maze with distracting interruption: possible implication for neural mechanisms of declarative and implicit long-term memory. Brain Research Bulletin 1998, 47, 249-256, 10.1016/s0361-9230(98)00083-5.
  10. Lauren J. Levy; Robert S. Astur; Karyn Frick; Men and Women Differ in Object Memory but Not Performance of a Virtual Radial Maze.. Behavioral Neuroscience 2005, 119, 853-862, 10.1037/0735-7044.119.4.853.
  11. Robert S Astur; Laughlin B Taylor; Adam N Mamelak; Linda Philpott; Robert J Sutherland; Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behavioural Brain Research 2001, 132, 77-84, 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00399-0.
  12. Laura Mandolesi; M. G. Leggio; A. Graziano; P. Neri; L. Petrosini; Cerebellar contribution to spatial event processing: involvement in procedural and working memory components. European Journal of Neuroscience 2001, 14, 2011-2022, 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01819.x.
  13. Laura Mandolesi; M. G. Leggio; F. Spirito; L. Petrosini; Cerebellar contribution to spatial event processing: do spatial procedures contribute to formation of spatial declarative knowledge?. European Journal of Neuroscience 2003, 18, 2618-2626, 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02990.x.
  14. L. Mandolesi; F. Addona; F. Foti; D. Menghini; L. Petrosini; S. Vicari; Spatial competences in Williams syndrome: a radial arm maze study. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 2009, 27, 205-213, 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2009.01.004.
  15. Aryeh Routtenberg; John O'keefe; Lynn Nadel; The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. The American Journal of Psychology 1980, 93, 177, 10.2307/1422119.
  16. Leonard E. Jarrard; On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. Behavioral and Neural Biology 1993, 60, 9-26, 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90664-4.
  17. Liana Palermo; Francesca Foti; Fabio Ferlazzo; Cecilia Guariglia; Laura Petrosini; I find my way in a maze but not in my own territory! Navigational processing in developmental topographical disorientation.. Neuropsychology 2014, 28, 135-146, 10.1037/neu0000021.
  18. Emily K. Farran; Harry R. M. Purser; Yannick Courbois; Marine Ballé; Pascal Sockeel; Daniel Mellier; Mark Blades; Route knowledge and configural knowledge in typical and atypical development: a comparison of sparse and rich environments.. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 2015, 7, 37, 10.1186/s11689-015-9133-6.
  19. Michael McLaren-Gradinaru; Ford Burles; Inderpreet Dhillon; Adam Retsinas; Alberto Umiltà; Jaimy Hannah; Kira Dolhan; Giuseppe Iaria; A Novel Training Program to Improve Human Spatial Orientation: Preliminary Findings. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2020, 14, 5, 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00005.
  20. Francesca Foti; Pierpaolo Sorrentino; Deny Menghini; Simone Montuori; Matteo Pesoli; Patrizia Turriziani; Stefano Vicari; Laura Petrosini; Laura Mandolesi; Peripersonal Visuospatial Abilities in Williams Syndrome Analyzed by a Table Radial Arm Maze Task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2020, 14, 254, 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00254.
  21. Andrea Serino; Peripersonal space (PPS) as a multisensory interface between the individual and the environment, defining the space of the self. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2019, 99, 138-159, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.016.
  22. Enrique Moraleda; Cristina Broglio; Fernando Rodríguez; Development of different spatial frames of reference for orientation in small-scale environments. Psicothema 2013, 25, 468-475, 10.7334/PSICOTHEMA2013.29.
  23. Nigel Foreman; Raphael Gillett; Sandra Jones; Choice autonomy and memory for spatial locations in six-year-old children. British Journal of Psychology 1994, 85, 17-27, 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02505.x.
  24. N Foreman; R Warry; P Murray; Development of reference and working spatial memory in preschool children.. The Journal of General Psychology 1990, 117, 267-276, .
  25. Francesca Foti; Deny Menghini; Laura Petrosini; Giuliana Valerio; Antonino Crinò; Stefano Vicari; Teresa Grimaldi; Laura Mandolesi; Spatial Competences in Prader–Willi Syndrome: A Radial Arm Maze Study. Behavior Genetics 2011, 41, 445-456, 10.1007/s10519-011-9471-4.
  26. Francesca Foti; Domenico Martone; Stefania Orrù; Simone Montuori; Esther Imperlini; Pasqualina Buono; Laura Petrosini; Laura Mandolesi; Are young children able to learn exploratory strategies by observation?. Psychological Research 2017, 82, 1212-1223, 10.1007/s00426-017-0896-0.
  27. Bohbot, V.D.; Jech, R.; Ruèicka, E.; Nadel, L.; Kanilna, M.; Stepankova, K.; Bures, J.; Rat Spatial Memory Tasks Adapted for Humans: Characterization in Subjects with Intact Brain and Subjects with Medial Temporal Lobe Lesions. Physiol. Res. 2002, 51, S49–S56, .
  28. Laura Serra; Fondazione Santa Lucia Neuroimaging Laboratory; Sara Raimondi; Carlotta di Domenico; Silvia Maffei; Anna Lardone; Marianna Liparoti; Pierpaolo Sorrentino; Carlo Caltagirone; Laura Petrosini; et al. The beneficial effects of physical exercise on visuospatial working memory in preadolescent children. AIMS Neuroscience 2021, 8, 496-509, 10.3934/neuroscience.2021026.
  29. Qazi Rahman; Johanna Koerting; Sexual orientation-related differences in allocentric spatial memory tasks. Hippocampus 2007, 18, 55-63, 10.1002/hipo.20375.
  30. Gaën Plancher; A. Tirard; V. Gyselinck; S. Nicolas; P. Piolino; Using virtual reality to characterize episodic memory profiles in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: Influence of active and passive encoding. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 592-602, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.013.
  31. Eleanor A. Maguire; Rory Nannery; Hugo Spiers; Navigation around London by a taxi driver with bilateral hippocampal lesions. Brain 2006, 129, 2894-2907, 10.1093/brain/awl286.
  32. Giuseppe Iaria; Michael Petrides; Alain Dagher; Bruce Pike; Véronique D. Bohbot; Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice. The Journal of Neuroscience 2003, 23, 5945-5952, 10.1523/jneurosci.23-13-05945.2003.
  33. Véronique D. Bohbot; Giuseppe Iaria; Michael Petrides; Hippocampal Function and Spatial Memory: Evidence From Functional Neuroimaging in Healthy Participants and Performance of Patients With Medial Temporal Lobe Resections.. Neuropsychology 2004, 18, 418-425, 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.418.
  34. Harrison Banner; Venkataramana Bhat; Nicole Etchamendy; Ridha Joober; Véronique D. Bohbot; The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism is associated with reduced functional magnetic resonance imaging activity in the hippocampus and increased use of caudate nucleus-dependent strategies in a human virtual navigation task. European Journal of Neuroscience 2011, 33, 968-977, 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07550.x.
  35. Rachel Marsh; Xuejun Hao; Dongrong Xu; Zhishun Wang; Yunsuo Duan; Jun Liu; Alayar Kangarlu; Diana Martinez; Felix Garcia; Gregory Z. Tau; et al. A virtual reality-based FMRI study of reward-based spatial learning. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 2912-2921, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.033.
  36. Naomi J. Goodrich-Hunsaker; Ramona O. Hopkins; Spatial memory deficits in a virtual radial arm maze in amnesic participants with hippocampal damage.. Behavioral Neuroscience 2010, 124, 405-413, 10.1037/a0019193.
  37. Véronique D. Bohbot; Jason Lerch; Brook Thorndycraft; Giuseppe Iaria; Alex P. Zijdenbos; Gray Matter Differences Correlate with Spontaneous Strategies in a Human Virtual Navigation Task. The Journal of Neuroscience 2007, 27, 10078-10083, 10.1523/jneurosci.1763-07.2007.
  38. Robert S Astur; Jennifer Tropp; Simona Sava; R.Todd Constable; Etan J Markus; Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation. Behavioural Brain Research 2004, 151, 103-115, 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.08.024.
  39. Veronique D. Bohbot; Sam McKenzie; Kyoko Konishi; Celine Fouquet; Vanessa Kurdi; Russell Schachar; Michel Boivin; Philippe Robaey; Virtual navigation strategies from childhood to senescence: evidence for changes across the life span. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 2012, 4, 28, 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00028.
  40. Julia Anglen Bauer; Birgit Claus Henn; Christine Austin; Silvia Zoni; Chiara Fedrighi; Giuseppa Cagna; Donatella Placidi; Roberta F. White; Qiong Yang; Brent A. Coull; et al. Manganese in teeth and neurobehavior: Sex-specific windows of susceptibility. Environment International 2017, 108, 299-308, 10.1016/j.envint.2017.08.013.
  41. Marilyn Cyr; Zhishun Wang; Gregory Z. Tau; Guihu Zhao; Eve Friedl; Mihaela Stefan; Kate Terranova; Rachel Marsh; Reward-Based Spatial Learning in Teens With Bulimia Nervosa. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2016, 55, 962-971.e3, 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.07.778.
  42. E.M. Migo; O. O’Daly; M. Mitterschiffthaler; E. Antonova; G.R. Dawson; C.T. Dourish; K.J. Craig; A. Simmons; G.K. Wilcock; E. McCulloch; et al. Investigating virtual reality navigation in amnestic mild cognitive impairment using fMRI. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 2015, 23, 196-217, 10.1080/13825585.2015.1073218.
  43. Philippe Robaey; Sam McKenzie; Russel Schachar; Michel Boivin; Veronique D. Bohbot; Stop and look! Evidence for a bias towards virtual navigation response strategies in children with ADHD symptoms. Behavioural Brain Research 2016, 298, 48-54, 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.08.019.
  44. Rachel Marsh; Gregory Z. Tau; Zhishun Wang; Yuankai Huo; Ge Liu; Xuejun Hao; Mark G. Packard; Bradley S. Peterson; H. Blair Simpson; Reward-Based Spatial Learning in Unmedicated Adults With Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 2015, 172, 383-392, 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121700.
  45. Jun-Young Lee; Sooyeon Kho; Hye Bin Yoo; Soowon Park; Jung-Seok Choi; Jun Soo Kwon; Kyung Ryeol Cha; Hee-Yeon Jung; Spatial memory impairments in amnestic mild cognitive impairment in a virtual radial arm maze. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2014, 10, 653-660, 10.2147/ndt.s58185.
  46. Eva Pirogovsky; Heather M. Holden; Cecily Jenkins; Guerry M. Peavy; David P. Salmon; Uglas R. Galasko; Paul E. Gilbert; Temporal sequence learning in healthy aging and amnestic mild cognitive impairment.. Experimental Aging Research 2013, 39, 371-81, 10.1080/0361073X.2013.808122.
  47. Leanne K. Wilkins; Todd A. Girard; Kyoko Konishi; Matthew King; Katherine A. Herdman; Jelena King; Bruce Christensen; Veronique D. Bohbot; Selective deficit in spatial memory strategies contrast to intact response strategies in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders tested in a virtual navigation task. Hippocampus 2013, 23, 1015-1024, 10.1002/hipo.22189.
  48. Elena A. Spieker; Robert S. Astur; Jeffrey T. West; Jacqueline A. Griego; Laura M. Rowland; Spatial memory deficits in a virtual reality eight-arm radial maze in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2012, 135, 84-89, 10.1016/j.schres.2011.11.014.
  49. Eva Pirogovsky; Jody Goldstein; Guerry Peavy; Mark W. Jacobson; Jody Corey-Bloom; Paul E. Gilbert; Temporal order memory deficits prior to clinical diagnosis in Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 2009, 15, 662-670, 10.1017/s1355617709990427.
  50. Federica Somma; Paolo Bartolomeo; Federica Vallone; Antonietta Argiuolo; Antonio Cerrato; Orazio Miglino; Laura Mandolesi; Maria Clelia Zurlo; Onofrio Gigliotta; Further to the left. Stress-induced increase of spatial pseudoneglect during the COVID-19 lockdown. null 2020, 12, 573846, 10.31234/osf.io/xb954.
  51. Hamed Taheri Gorji; Michela Leocadi; Francesco Grassi; Gaspare Galati; The art gallery maze: a novel tool to assess human navigational abilities. Cognitive Processing 2021, 22, 501-514, 10.1007/s10339-021-01022-9.
  52. Elza Rechtman; Paul Curtin; Demetrios M. Papazaharias; Stefano Renzetti; Giuseppa Cagna; Marco Peli; Yuri Levin-Schwartz; Donatella Placidi; Donald R. Smith; Roberto G. Lucchini; et al. Sex-specific associations between co-exposure to multiple metals and visuospatial learning in early adolescence. Translational Psychiatry 2020, 10, 1-10, 10.1038/s41398-020-01041-8.
  53. Devin J. Sodums; Véronique D. Bohbot; Negative correlation between grey matter in the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in healthy aging. Hippocampus 2020, 30, 892-908, 10.1002/hipo.23210.
  54. Louisa Dahmani; Blandine Courcot; Jamie Near; Raihaan Patel; Robert S. C. Amaral; M. Mallar Chakravarty; Véronique D. Bohbot; Fimbria-Fornix Volume Is Associated With Spatial Memory and Olfactory Identification in Humans. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 2020, 13, 87, 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00087.
  55. Jarid Goodman; Mason McClay; Joseph E. Dunsmoor; Threat-induced modulation of hippocampal and striatal memory systems during navigation of a virtual environment. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 2020, 168, 107160-107160, 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107160.
  56. Yingying Yang; Edward C. Merrill; Qi Wang; Children’s response, landmark, and metric strategies in spatial navigation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 2019, 181, 75-101, 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.01.005.
  57. Jeremy B Caplan; Eric Lg Legge; Bevin Cheng; Christopher R Madan; Effectiveness of the method of loci is only minimally related to factors that should influence imagined navigation.. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2019, 72, 2541-2553, 10.1177/1747021819858041.
  58. Étienne Aumont; Martin Arguin; Véronique Bohbot; Greg L. West; Increased flanker task and forward digit span performance in caudate-nucleus-dependent response strategies. Brain and Cognition 2019, 135, 103576, 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.014.
  59. Étienne Aumont; Caroll-Ann Blanchette; Veronique D. Bohbot; Greg L. West; Caudate nucleus-dependent navigation strategies are associated with increased risk-taking and set-shifting behavior. Learning & Memory 2019, 26, 101-108, 10.1101/lm.048306.118.
  60. Somayeh Raiesdana; Modeling the interaction of navigational systems in a reward-based virtual navigation task. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience 2018, 17, 45-67, 10.3233/JIN-170036.
  61. Louisa Dahmani; Raihaan M. Patel; Yiling Yang; M. Mallar Chakravarty; Lesley K. Fellows; Véronique D. Bohbot; An intrinsic association between olfactory identification and spatial memory in humans. Nature Communications 2018, 9, 1-12, 10.1038/s41467-018-06569-4.
  62. Étienne Aumont; Veronique D. Bohbot; Gregory L. West; Spatial learners display enhanced oculomotor performance. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 2018, 30, 872-879, 10.1080/20445911.2018.1526178.
  63. Kyoko Konishi; Ridha Joober; Judes Poirier; Kathleen MacDonald; Mallar Chakravarty; Raihaan Patel; John Breitner; Véronique D. Bohbot; Healthy versus Entorhinal Cortical Atrophy Identification in Asymptomatic APOE4 Carriers at Risk for Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 2018, 61, 1493-1507, 10.3233/JAD-170540.
  64. Hyunjeong Kim; Jin Young Park; Kwanguk (Kenny) Kim; Spatial Learning and Memory Using a Radial Arm Maze with a Head-Mounted Display. Psychiatry Investigation 2018, 15, 935-944, 10.30773/pi.2018.06.28.3.
  65. Tavor Ben-Zeev; Inbal Weiss; Saar Ashri; Yuval Heled; Itay Ketko; Ran Yanovich; Eitan Okun; Mild Physical Activity Does Not Improve Spatial Learning in a Virtual Environment. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 2020, 14, 584052, 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.584052.
More
This entry is offline, you can click here to edit this entry!
Video Production Service