4. Perceptual and Attentional Asymmetries for Human Bodies
In more recent years, the DVF paradigm was also introduced in the study of human body parts, and in particular hands. Specifically, the lateralized presentation of bodies and body parts has been suggested as a way to study hemispheric asymmetries in motor representations
[56][57][58][105,106,107]. For instance, it has been shown that participants respond faster when left and right hand stimuli are presented to the ipsilateral hemifield/contralateral hemisphere than when they are presented to the contralateral hemifield/ipsilateral hemisphere
[56][105]. Moreover, Parsons et al.
[58][107] found that callosotomy patients were faster and more accurate in judging the laterality of both left and right hand stimuli when they were presented to the ipsilateral hemifield/contralateral hemisphere than when they were presented to the contralateral hemifield/ipsilateral hemisphere (similar results were observed in healthy controls). In agreement with such findings, de Lussanet et al.
[57][106] suggested that each hemisphere contains better visuo-motor representations for the contralateral body side than for the ipsilateral body side. Specifically, these
scautho
lars showed that—compared with leftward-facing point-light walkers (PLWs)—rightward-facing PLWs were recognized better in the RVF, whereas—compared with rightward-facing PLWs—leftward-facing PLWs were recognized better in the LVF. In other words, compared with PLWs facing toward the point of gaze, those facing away from the point of gaze appeared more vivid. Such a lateralized facing effect was explained by de Lussanet et al.
[57][106] by proposing that the visual perception of lateralized body stimuli is facilitated when the corresponding visual and body representations are located in the same hemisphere (given the contralateral organization of both the visual and motor-somatosensory systems). Actually, this is true when a PLW faces away from the observer’s fixation point, so that a lateralized embodiment of the observed body is fostered because the hemibody seen in the foreground is processed by the sensory-motor cortex located in the same side as the visual cortex processing the stimulus.
It should be noticed that asymmetries in the perception of human bodies or body parts have also been reported in studies that do not resort to the DVF paradigm. Specifically, various studies investigating the perception of sport actions showed that the result of right limb actions is anticipated better than that of left limb actions
[59][60][61][62][63][64][65][112,113,114,115,116,117,118]. As suggested by Hagemann
[59][112] and Loffing et al.
[61][114] (see also
[60][62][63][65][66][113,115,116,118,119]), the ability to discriminate actions performed with the left hand is less developed than that to discriminate actions performed with the right hand. This is consistent with the advantage that left-handers and left-footers exhibit in several interactive sports
[66][67][68][69][70][71][72][73][74][75][76][77][78][79][80][81][82][83][119,120,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129,130,131,132,133,134,135,136].
5. Asymmetries in Social Touch
Many other examples of behavioral asymmetries can be found during social interactions among humans, and are mainly observed in complex motor activities such as embracing, kissing and infant-holding, wherein the motor behavior shared reciprocally by two persons entails necessarily a sensory counterpart, social touch
[6][84][6,142]. Relatively few studies have systematically investigated the first two instances of interactive social touch, showing a substantial rightward asymmetry for both embracing
[85][86][143,144] and kissing
[87][145], with the latter finding being considered as more controversial (e.g., see
[88][146]). As regards infant-holding, the left-cradling bias (LCB: the tendency to hold infants predominantly using the left rather than the right arm
[89][147];
Figure 24) has received much more scholarly attention over the last 60 years. Although this lateralized behavior,
differe
ferntly from those reviewed above, refers to a motor rather than perceptual asymmetry, it nonetheless entails dealing with a human social stimulus (the infant) and seems to be related as well to perceptual asymmetries for social/emotional stimuli. Accordingly,
there iswe argue that a guiding thread exists between the aforementioned LVF advantage for faces, the higher social salience of infant facial features found in women than in men
[90][91][92][148,149,150], and the left-sided infant positioning during cradling interactions being shown to a greater extent by women than by men
[93][151]. First of all, it should be noticed that a fairly robust LCB has been shown—regardless of assessment methodologies—both in left-handed women (and men, although to a lesser degree
[94][95][152,153]) and in a mother affected by situs inversus with dextrocardia (i.e., a condition in which the heart is atypically placed in the right rather than the left side of the chest
[96][154]). Therefore, the two first explanations proposed, namely the “handedness” (i.e., cradling infants with the non-dominant hand would free the dominant arm for other tasks
[97][155]) and “heartbeat” (i.e., cradling infants on the left side would enhance the soothing effect of the mother’s heartbeat sound
[89][147]) hypotheses cannot be accepted as reliable accounts of the LCB. On the contrary, it is now believed that the LCB is due to a population-level right-hemispheric dominance for socio-emotional processing, as suggested by several studies carried out in this particular field over the last three decades (e.g.,
[98][156]). For example, Harris et al.
[99][157] used the chimeric face paradigm in order to reveal the relationship between participants’ right-hemispheric specialization for processing facial emotion and their lateral cradling preference, as assessed by means of an imagination task. These
scautho
lars found that participants who imagined holding the infant on the left side showed a stronger LVF advantage (i.e., judged as more expressive the chimeras in which the emotional half was on the left side of the face) compared with participants who imagined holding the infant on the right side. Bourne and Todd
[100][158] confirmed this finding using the chimeric face paradigm as well and a life-like doll to assess participants’ cradling lateral preferences. Consistent findings were reported by Vauclair and Donnot
[101][159], who used a similar methodology (chimeric face paradigm and doll cradling task), although only in women.
Figure 24.
Example of left-cradling bias (LCB).