2. CT Fibers: The Discovery
Neurophysiological and neuroanatomical studies have demonstrated that discriminative and affiliative types of touch are encoded in two different ways. Traditionally, research on touch has mostly focused on the class of receptors responsible for transducing information about pressure/vibration, temperature, itch, and pain, i.e., the discriminative system. In comparison, the affective system has been particularly studied in recent decades.
CT fibers were first identified in cats in 1939, and they have been found in different species and body sites ever since (i.e., mice, guinea pigs, rats, pigs, and nonhuman primates)
[15,16,17,18,19][15][16][17][18][19]. As they were identified in humans only in 1988, for a long time, it was believed that these fibers had disappeared in humans for an evolutionary reason. Subsequently, they were found in the infraorbital nerve, the hairy side of the arm, and the legs
[20,21][20][21]. Therefore, the idea that CT fibers were absent in humans due to being unnecessary, was overruled and then replaced by research on the skin and the importance of emotional valence of touch, as well as by evidence that some disorders (e.g., depression) have deficits in this social system.
WResearche
rs will deepen the discussion on this topic in the “Social Touch and Its Implication for Neurologic and Psychiatric Disorders” section. A critical year is 2010, when Morrison and colleagues
[22] proposed the hypothesis “skin as a social organ”, which gives CT fibers the role of transmission and processing the social dimension of specific touch. Olausson
[23] elaborated on the social touch hypothesis during the same period.
3. Properties of CT Fibers
Animal and human studies conducted with von Frey monofilaments have established the CT fibers’ physiological properties. Their receptive fields are round/oval and consist of one to nine small responsive spots distributed over an area up to 35 mm
2 [20], and they respond to innocuous dynamic stimulation across the hairy skin surface
[24,25][24][25]. The stimulus must be characterized by a force in the range of 0.22–2.5 mN and a velocity of 1–10 cm/s. Velocities lower than 1 cm/s or higher than 10 cm/s lead to a decrease in their firing frequency. This typical response to stimuli has the characteristic upside-down U-firing shape. Once activated, the fibers produce high-frequency trains of action potentials (50–100 impulses/s), with a peak rate of 100 impulses/s. A crucial feature to be considered is that CT fibers are unmyelinated, meaning they have slow conduction velocity (0.6–1.3 m/s)
[24]. This aspect cannot be correlated with the discriminative aspect of the touch because
wresearche
rs need fast speed in order to discriminate the features of the touched object. Indeed, Aβ fibers are myelinated and have these functions. By contrast, CT fibers are not myelinated.
4. Effects of CT Fibers on the Autonomous Nervous System
At the peripheral skin level, it has been demonstrated that CT stimulation evokes a sympathetic skin response. In studies in which skin resistance changes were registered with a constant current electrodermal recording device, using Ag–AgCl electrodes placed on the glabrous skin of the nondominant hand, CT activation was shown to trigger sympathetic arousal
[26,27][26][27]. Moreover, it has been shown that the stimulation of CT fibers also modulates the level of stress hormones and positively modulates the parasympathetic system. The vagal tone is the activity of the vagus nerve, which is a fundamental component of the parasympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system. The vagus nerve regulates the automatic systems, and therefore different situations, e.g., anxiety or stress. The vagal tone can be measured by heart rate variability, i.e., the balance between sympathetic and parasympathetic activity in the body in a resting state but also different conditions
[28,29][28][29].
In humans, it has been demonstrated that among self-stroking, stroking, and being stroked by a partner, just the last condition has a significant autonomic effect, such as the decrement in heart rate
[30], even though all of them are stimuli rated as pleasant. In this experiment, the stroke was applied with speed and pressure optimal for the modulation of CT fibers. No visual feedback was provided. Thus, participants could not see each other during the task. An interesting conclusion of the authors is that the absence of the role of the autonomic system during stroking could be due to the absence of visual feedback and because of the absence of CT fibers on the glabrous side of the palm. Moreover, both self-stroking and stroking are perceived as less pleasant than being stroked based on (1) the mechanisms to distinguish self-touch and externally produced touch and (2) the mechanism according to which
wresearche
rs correctly predict the pleasantness of the self-touch
[30].
A great effect of affective touch is a reduction in pain in healthy subjects
[31] and in some diseases, such as chronic pain. Chronic pain is defined as pain for 3 consecutive months even after treatment
[32]. Nowadays, the treatment combines pharmacological and physical rehabilitation, but the combination is often insufficient to resolve the problem. New therapies are necessary. Larissa L. Meijer and colleagues
[33] reported the effect of affective touch on a 73-year-old woman with a chronic pain diagnosis. Affective touch was applied with a speed of around 3 cm/s, i.e., the optimal speed to modulate CT fibers, and nonaffective touch was applied with a speed of 18 cm/s, outside the range to modulate CT fibers. The patient reported that from the third to the seventh day, the pain disappeared. Importantly, this was not reported during the nonaffective touch treatment.
Another effect of affective touch is the modulation of hormones and neuropeptides. In chimpanzees, grooming with a bond partner increase the urinary secretion of oxytocin
[34], supporting the fact that the affective touch can contribute to the production of such neuropeptide. On the other hand, several findings demonstrate that hormones and neuropeptides can regulate the production and perception of the social touch. It is known that oxytocin is one critical player in affiliative behaviors. Since 1980, research has highlighted the potent effect of this neuropeptide on social interactions in both humans and animals. Drago and colleagues
[35] demonstrated that the intracerebroventricular injection of oxytocin induces an increment in grooming in rats. The authors have hypothesized that oxytocin increased affiliative behavior through central mechanisms, in particular through dopamine and opioid systems. Indeed, the opiate receptor antagonist naloxone and the dopamine antagonist haloperidol decreased or suppressed the grooming behavior, respectively. In a study involving female bonobos, oxytocin administration increased grooming behaviors, even if the authors of the study underline that interindividual differences are likely
[36]. In humans, the administration of oxytocin may regulate the social evaluations of others after being touched
[37]. In the same study, the authors demonstrated an interaction between touch and facial expressions, with angry faces negatively affecting the rating of touch pleasantness
[37]. Another study demonstrated that the intranasal application of oxytocin to humans at a resting state induces an increment in heart rate variability, an index of the vagal tone
[38].
Finally, another crucial aspect of social touch is the self/other touch distinction since it can be dysfunctional in some neurological diseases. Indeed, differences in the cortical and spinal modulation during self- and other touch have been investigated. Interestingly, researchers have found important differences in the modulation of the insula and the anterior cingulate cortex during these two conditions
[39].
5. Social Touch and Mirror Neurons
In the last few years, several studies have tried to reveal neural mechanisms for perceiving and understanding social interactions
[52][40]. Understanding the conspecific’s experiences is crucial for social behavior, and according to the mirror neuron theory, this understanding is accomplished by an internal simulation of other’s experiences we are observing
[53][41].
Mirror neurons, discovered originally in monkey brains and later in human brains, were first described in a seminal paper in 1992 as a class of monkey premotor cells discharging during action execution and observation
[54][42]. Rozzi and colleagues
[55][43] recorded these neurons also from the cortex of the inferior parietal lobule of macaques. Subsequently, mirror-like neurons were found in different brain areas and animal species (rodents, birds, bats).
After the discovery of mirror neurons for body actions, it has been advanced the hypothesis that an analogous mechanism could be involved in the observation of tactile stimulations. Goldman and Gallese were the first to hypothesize a somatosensory mirror mechanism, which would allow observers to map the observed tactile stimulations onto their somatosensory system
[56][44]. Subsequently, attesting to the social importance of touch, mirror neuron-type responses to observed touch have been reported within the same neural regions activating when the touch is experienced first-hand
[57][45]. Several studies suggest that the posterior portion of the human secondary somatosensory cortex plays a key role in mirror-touch synesthesia
[58][46]. Imaging studies (with functional magnetic resonance imaging) showed that observing another person being touched activates brain areas, such as the primary and secondary somatosensory areas and premotor cortex, which are normally activated when an individual’s body is touched
[57][45].
Functionally, the somatosensory cortex has been linked to empathic ability
[59][47], the recognition of emotional expressions
[60][48], and the affective valence and intensity of the observed social touch, such as caressing and slapping someone else’s hand. Intriguingly, observing these last two stimuli more strongly activates primary and secondary somatosensory areas than the observation of a simple contact without affective connotation
[61][49].
The insula cortex is also a region of interest in affective mechanisms
[62][50]. Evidence on affective touch in humans suggests that the insular cortex could be an interface between exteroceptive and interoceptive perceptions of social touch during skin-to-skin communication. Morrison et al.
[12] showed that the responses to dynamic stroking touch can be “velocity-tuned” and “socially specific”. They found that the posterior insula cortex was most activated for CT optimal velocity social stroking rather than non-CT optimal velocities or nonsocial dynamic touch. Evidence supports that individual differences in vicarious responses to touch exist due to personality traits or cognitive state
[63,64][51][52]. Furthermore, the link between tactile experience and vicarious responding is tangible in patients carrying a heritable mutation, resulting in reduced C-fiber density. This, in turn, implies that these patients not only evaluate the directly experienced CT-related optimal touch as less pleasant
[65][53] but also feel a smaller sense of gratification from observing the same touch compared with control subjects
[11].
Empirical evidence shows that the social touch can affect the way we mentally represent our body, and it does this in a somatotopic manner
[66][54]. In addition, the observation of tactile stimulation delivered to a virtual hand is associated with changes in brain activity related to vicarious somatosensation
[67][55]. This last study shows how changes in body ownership can affect the neural activity of brain structures underlying vicarious somatosensation. It is noteworthy that, among those brain regions that changed their neural activity during the vicarious somatosensation, there was also the insula, which not only is the target of CT fibers but is also known to be involved in the processing of somatosensory aspects of altered body ownership
[67][55].
Finally, it seems fair to say that the mirror mechanism allows a translation of the perceived action, regardless of whether we have seen, heard, or felt it, into the identical motor representation of the given action goal, resulting in an embodied link between two individuals
[68][56]. The motor resonance mechanism of mirror neurons most likely represents the neural correlate of understanding others’ actions and intentions, which has been described as “embodied simulation”
[69,70,71,72][57][58][59][60]. However, actions are not the unique experiences characterizing interpersonal relationships, which instead imply also sharing affective states, such as emotions and sensations. It is well known that the same brain structures involved in the experience of emotions and sensations are also activated when the same emotions and sensations are recognized in others. Several “mirroring” mechanisms exist in human brains and, thanks to the “intentional consonance”
[73][61], these mechanisms allow us to experience intersubjective relationships and empathize with others.
Overall, several studies have described mirror neurons and mirror-like responses not only during the execution/observation of actions but also for many other kinds of stimuli, such as tactile stimulations, showing that merely viewing touch involves the observers’ somatosensory cortices. There is a large body of evidence that indicates that information about others’ actions, emotions, sensations, and communicative messages are mapped onto the same beholder’s neural substrates devoted to those first-person processes
[74][62]. Therefore, it would appear that the mirror mechanism allows for a basic widespread remapping of other-related information belonging to a large variety of domains (especially social cognition and social behavior) onto primarily self-related brain structures. Notably, a few of these structures, which are involved in the processing of emotions, belong to the mirror network, and they are also targets of CT fibers.
6. Social Touch and Mirror Neurons
In the last few years, several studies have tried to reveal neural mechanisms for perceiving and understanding social interactions
[52][40]. Understanding the conspecific’s experiences is crucial for social behavior, and according to the mirror neuron theory, this understanding is accomplished by an internal simulation of other’s experiences we are observing
[53][41].
Overall, several studies have described mirror neurons and mirror-like responses not only during the execution/observation of actions but also for many other kinds of stimuli, such as tactile stimulations, showing that merely viewing touch involves the observers’ somatosensory cortices. There is a large body of evidence that indicates that information about others’ actions, emotions, sensations, and communicative messages are mapped onto the same beholder’s neural substrates devoted to those first-person processes
[74][62]. Therefore, it would appear that the mirror mechanism allows for a basic widespread remapping of other-related information belonging to a large variety of domains (especially social cognition and social behavior) onto primarily self-related brain structures. Notably, a few of these structures, which are involved in the processing of emotions, belong to the mirror network, and they are also targets of CT fibers.
7. From Social Brain to Social Behavior
Neuroanatomical studies have been performed on areas where neurons with mirror properties have been found. Through those studies,
wresearche
rs can identify two different mirror neuron networks: sensorimotor and emotional (
Figure 1 A)
.
tThe network underlying affective touch (
Figure 1B) includes the thalamus, the primary somatosensory cortex, the insular cortex, the medial prefrontal cortex, and the amygdala. Interestingly, the insular cortex is the cortical node of both mirror neurons and affective touch systems. Additionally, it is known that, in neurotypical individuals, CT optimal touch activates social brain networks, and they include, as
wresearche
rs already underlined, the bilateral insula together with the parietal operculum, the orbitofrontal cortex, the primary somatosensory cortex, the posterior superior temporal sulcus, and the inferior frontal gyrus
[93,94,95,96][63][64][65][66].
Figure 1. Neural networks of mirror neuron system and affective touch: (A) Organization of primate sensorimotor (purple) and emotional (green) mirror neuron networks based on macaque neuroanatomical studies on areas in which neurons with mirror properties have been found. The sensorimotor network includes, beyond the ventral premotor cortex (PMv) and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL), the primary motor cortex (M1), the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), the mesial premotor cortex (PMm), the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the anterior intraparietal area (AIP), and the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII). The emotional network includes the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), amygdala (A), and insula (IC). Abbreviations: P, principal sulcus; AI, arcuate inferior sulcus; arcuate superior sulcus; L, lateral sulcus; C, central sulcus; ST, superior temporal sulcus; Lu, lunatu. (B) The circuit of affective touch includes external tactile stimuli, which produce mechanical energy. This mechanical energy, received at the somatosensory nerve endings in the skin, is transduced and transmitted via CT fibers up through lamina I and to thalamic nuclei (i.e., ventromedial posterior nucleus, ventral posterior inferior nucleus) and processed at the insula (IC), before reaching the social centers in the cortex (e.g., medial prefrontal cortex, mPFC) and amygdala. Abbreviations: S1, primary somatosensory cortex.
8. Social Touch and Its Implication for Neurologic and Psychiatric Disorders
A strong body of evidence supports the finding that social touch is relevant to several neurological and psychiatric disorders since CT fibers’ dysfunction appears involved in these pathologies. For example, it has been demonstrated that, in patients displaying a congenitally reduced density of unmyelinated sensory fibers, the pIC does not activate when a slow and gentle touch is applied. This is most likely due to poor CT afferent inputs to the cortex
[12].
Anorexia nervosa is an eating disorder characterized by abnormally low body weight, an intense fear of gaining weight, and a distorted perception of the body (overestimation of body size and shape). People with anorexia place a high value on controlling their weight and shape, using extreme efforts that tend to significantly interfere with their lives. Importantly, these patients have deficits in interoceptive processing and their internal physiological condition interpretation, creating and maintaining a mismatch between internal and external bodily sensations. Some studies have reported that anorexia nervosa patients perceive touch that activates CT fibers as less pleasant than control healthy individuals
[102][67].
Croy and colleagues
[102][67] investigated this topic in patients with one or more psychiatric disorders (somatoform disorders, post-traumatic stress disorders, anxiety, somatoform disorders, and mood and affective disorders). Patients reported significantly fewer interpersonal touch episodes compared with a healthy control group, regardless of whether they lived alone or not. Moreover, patients differed from the control group in terms of touch pleasantness but not regarding its awareness.
Finally, there are a few clinical conditions (e.g., autism spectrum disorder) related not only to CT fibers but also to the mirror neuron system.
Altogether, the data available supporting the fact that subjects with autism spectrum disorder have a typical theory of mind
[108][68], suggest that the social impairments displayed by these people are more related to the inability to “experience” the socioemotional attributes of affective touch rather than the inability to “interpret” the experiences of others
[109][69].
9. Touch as Therapy?
Human studies have shown that affiliative touch and massage could be useful in several clinical pictures. Recently, a meta-analysis on the effect of therapeutic massage on symptoms of Parkinson’s disease provided evidence that therapeutic massage could alleviate the typical motor symptoms of this illness, improving motor functions even though it did not ameliorate the quality of daily life of patients in comparison to healthy subjects
[110][70]. In the reviewed studies in this meta-analysis, the applied massages were traditional Chinese Tuina, a common massage technique targeting the limbs of patients; acupressure; and Thai massage.
Recently, Tsuji and colleagues analyzed the salivary oxytocin concentration in children with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder when receiving a massage from their mothers
[115][71].
Lately, mechanical affective touch therapy has been proposed as a useful treatment for controlling symptoms of anxiety disorders. The applied stimulation through mechanical affective touch therapy has CT fibers as its target. Electroencephalography studies revealed that the treatment also led to increased occipital theta and alpha oscillatory activity
[116][72].
Preterm newborns represent another patient population for whom CT-related touch is increasingly used as a treatment. There is evidence that tactile stimulation improves the immune system in premature newborns
[118][73].
The International Workshop on Kangaroo Mother Care, 2009, recommends the implementation of continuous kangaroo mother care as the gold standard pervading all medical and nursing care, based on empirical studies and clinical guidelines.
Altogether, these studies emphasize how several touch techniques (i.e., psychoactive massage therapy) have been introduced into different fields, from the treatment of psychiatric disorders (depression, anxiety, or psychosomatic disorders) to the treatment of chronic pain, such as in patients with cancer
[123][74].
That said,
wresearche
rs believe there are different motivations to encourage future investigations in academic medicine, clinical psychology, and basic research to take steps to make touch therapy attractive and accepted as an affective-oriented assessment and intervention, resulting in a more precise and standardized classification of clinical conditions affecting emotional processing.
10. Conclusions
Because of the critical role of social touch in human well-being and the evidence supporting its use as a treatment for both newborns and adults, more detailed studies are necessary. Indeed, investigating the correlation between each disorder and the corresponding CT dysfunction and/or distorted perception of affiliative touch should help to develop new treatments based on massage and CT-fiber stimulation in association with pharmacological therapies.
Ultimately, it would be useful to develop new therapies based on the protocols targeting CT fibers in order to alleviate symptoms in several clinical conditions. These treatments should improve not only behavioral symptoms, as seen in autistic children, but also the autonomic state. Importantly, new investigations might underline the possible effect of CT stimulation in pathological conditions in terms of circuit modulation in the “social brain”.