Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 -- 1154 2023-12-22 16:58:56 |
2 Reference format revised. Meta information modification 1154 2023-12-25 02:18:51 |

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Wei, S.; Lei, Q.; Chen, Y.; Xin, Y.P. Visual Cueing and Eye-Tracking Technology in Education. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/53076 (accessed on 29 April 2024).
Wei S, Lei Q, Chen Y, Xin YP. Visual Cueing and Eye-Tracking Technology in Education. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/53076. Accessed April 29, 2024.
Wei, Shuang, Qingli Lei, Yingjie Chen, Yan Ping Xin. "Visual Cueing and Eye-Tracking Technology in Education" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/53076 (accessed April 29, 2024).
Wei, S., Lei, Q., Chen, Y., & Xin, Y.P. (2023, December 22). Visual Cueing and Eye-Tracking Technology in Education. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/53076
Wei, Shuang, et al. "Visual Cueing and Eye-Tracking Technology in Education." Encyclopedia. Web. 22 December, 2023.
Visual Cueing and Eye-Tracking Technology in Education
Edit

Visual cueing has been widely utilized in the practice of online learning. Visual cues like bold text, colors, arrows, or underlining are used to highlight important information in online materials. Online multimedia resources like video may use visual cues like blinking, highlighting, or zooming in on important details. Eye-tracking data can provide valuable insights into learners’ visual behaviors, revealing what they are looking at, how long they focus on specific elements, and the sequences of their gazes’ movements across different representations. 

Visual Cueing Eye-Tracking Technology Education

1. Introduction

Visual cues are extensively employed in the realm of education, ranging from color-coded reading materials to highlighted multimedia content [1]. People commonly use visual cues with the presumption that they positively influence students’ learning. However, a critical question arises: is this presumption supported by evidence? Researchers have been exploring the effects of visual cues on learning for several decades, expanding their exploration to encompass various learning environments along with the advancement of learning technology—from paper books to multimedia material and from online education to virtual-reality education. Their findings suggested that visual cues effectively direct students’ attention and facilitate their information-searching processes [2][3]. However, despite these promising results, it is essential to acknowledge that there is still no consensus on whether visual cues truly lead to a significant improvement in student performance.
In some studies, students achieved better performance, which was explained by the impact of visual cues in guiding students’ attention away from irrelevant areas and saving their working memory for the key information, consequently leading to better learning results [4]. However, in some other studies, students only scored well in retention tasks. In transfer tasks, students’ performance was not better [5][6], indicating that visual cues are only external models; students need to understand the information and build their own internal models to solve transfer tasks. As external tools, visual cues play a limited role in helping students understand, internalize, and apply information [5]. Some scholars’ studies even denied the role of visual cues in helping students to focus on information. They pointed out that students may just be guided and not really see the information [7]

2. Visual Cueing in Online Learning

Visual cueing has been widely utilized in the practice of online learning. Visual cues like bold text, colors, arrows, or underlining are used to highlight important information in online materials [8][9]. Online multimedia resources like video may use visual cues like blinking, highlighting, or zooming in on important details [10][11]. The effects of visual cueing have been extensively investigated across various aspects, including different learning subjects (e.g., science, reading, problem solving, etc.) and diverse student groups (e.g., children, university students, low-performing students, etc.). Numerous studies have demonstrated the attention-cueing effect of visual cues [12][13]. For example, Jamet [6] employed an eye-tracking method to explore the impact of visual cues on students’ problem-solving processes and found that visual cues effectively redirected students’ attention toward relevant information while reducing their focus on unrelated areas. This allowed students to allocate more working memory to process the relevant information, leading to improved performance in retention tasks. However, the effect on transfer tasks was not significant. Moreover, the use of visual cues has been associated with a reduction in off-task behaviors in many studies, as students’ attention is effectively directed to the related information area. A study conducted by Kercood et al. [14] implemented text highlighting and observed that it increased problem-solving time for females at risk for ADHD, decreased off-task duration, and improved the females’ problem-solving performance. In addition to attention cueing and off-task duration decrease, visual cueing has demonstrated a third important effect—information-searching. In a study by Ozcelik et al. [4], color was employed in multimedia learning, resulting in an increased performance of participants in both retention and transfer tasks. They analyzed students’ eye movements during problem-solving processes and concluded that the use of color coding enhanced learning by facilitating the efficient location of corresponding information in illustrations and text.
Content-independent visual cueing has been studied, and three positive effects have been consistently verified, on attention cueing, off-task reduction, and information searching. However, beyond these effects, the role of visual cueing in facilitating understanding and improving performance remains a contentious topic. While some of the mentioned studies demonstrated that students achieved better performance with the help of visual cueing, not all research supports this finding. For example, Yeari et al. [3] conducted a study wherein text highlighting guided students’ attention to the central text area, but no significant difference in students’ processing and recall of central information was observed across the highlighted conditions. Cojean and Jamet [5] also verified the effect of visual cueing on information seeking, but they did not find any promotion of information understanding. They suggested that visual cues act as external cues, guiding students to the information but not necessarily ensuring its internalization. Similarly, Yang [10] reached a similar conclusion, noting that visual cueing effectively guides attention but may not optimize conceptual understanding or lead to improved performance. Furthermore, they have found evidence of a learning-interference effect of visual cueing on high-performing students, suggesting that for certain groups of learners, visual cueing may not be as beneficial and could potentially hinder their learning processes.

3. Eye-Tracking Technology in Education

The application of eye-tracking technology in educational settings dates back to the 1980s and 1990s, when researchers began using eye-tracking data to understand and analyze learners’ comprehension processes when dealing with textual and pictorial information [15][16][17]. Van Gog and Scheiter [18] emphasized that eye-tracking data can provide valuable insights into learners’ visual behaviors, revealing what they are looking at, how long they focus on specific elements, and the sequences of their gazes’ movements across different representations. Eye trackers measure various eye-movement parameters, such as fixation duration, gaze duration, and transition frequency, to capture these behaviors objectively (for a comprehensive list of eye-movement metrics, please refer to the paper by Bednarik et al. [19]).
Researchers have used eye-tracking technology to investigate various aspects of learning behavior. For example, Brunyé and Taylor [20] explored how different learning goals may impact learners’ outcomes. Schwonke et al. [21] observed changes in attentional behaviors when learners interacted with different representations and how these behaviors influenced learning. Eye-movement data have been deemed more objective and reliable than subjective self-reports [22].
Beyond attention, researchers have attempted to connect eye movements with other learning behaviors. Just and Carpenter [16] proposed the eye–mind hypothesis, suggesting that the eyes fixate on the areas where the mind is actively engaged. She and Chen [23] compared the effects of different multimedia methods on students’ eye fixation behaviors and empirically verified a direct correlation between the duration of eye fixations and the depth of learning. Wu et al. [24] adopted eye-movement measures, such as total fixation duration, number of long fixations, and pupil size, to predict participants’ performance. Susac et al. [22] analyzed students’ eye fixations, reaction time, performance, and questionnaires to reveal their strategies in equation solving. Eye-tracking technology has been widely applied in online-learning research and is recognized as a powerful tool to study student learning behavior.

References

  1. Park, S. An Exploratory Study on the Meaning of Visual Scaffolding in Teaching and Learning Contexts. Educ. Technol. Int. 2017, 18, 215–247.
  2. Liu, R.; Xu, X.; Yang, H.; Li, Z.; Huang, G. Impacts of Cues on Learning and Attention in Immersive 360-Degree Video: An Eye-Tracking Study. Front. Psychol. 2022, 12, 792069.
  3. Yeari, M.; Oudega, M.; van den Broek, P. The Effect of Highlighting on Processing and Memory of Central and Peripheral Text Information: Evidence from Eye Movements. J. Res. Read. 2017, 40, 365–383.
  4. Ozcelik, E.; Karakus, T.; Kursun, E.; Cagiltay, K. An Eye-Tracking Study of How Color Coding Affects Multimedia Learning. Comput. Educ. 2009, 53, 445–453.
  5. Cojean, S.; Jamet, E. The Role of Scaffolding in Improving Information Seeking in Videos. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 2018, 34, 960–969.
  6. Jamet, E. An Eye-Tracking Study of Cueing Effects in Multimedia Learning. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014, 32, 47–53.
  7. de Koning, B.B.; Tabbers, H.K.; Rikers, R.M.J.P.; Paas, F. Attention Guidance in Learning from a Complex Animation: Seeing Is Understanding? Learn. Instr. 2010, 20, 111–122.
  8. Jamet, E.; Gavota, M.; Quaireau, C. Attention Guiding in Multimedia Learning. Learn. Instr. 2008, 18, 135–145.
  9. Dunlosky, J.; Rawson, K.A.; Marsh, E.J.; Nathan, M.J.; Willingham, D.T. Improving Students’ Learning with Effective Learning Techniques: Promising Directions from Cognitive and Educational Psychology. Psychol. Sci. Pub. Int. Suppl. 2013, 14, 4–58.
  10. Yang, H.Y. The Effects of Attention Cueing on Visualizers’ Multimedia Learning. Educ. Technol. Soc. 2016, 19, 249–262.
  11. Glaser, M.; Lengyel, D.; Toulouse, C.; Schwan, S. Designing Computer-Based Learning Contents: Influence of Digital Zoom on Attention. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 2017, 65, 1135–1151.
  12. Scheiter, K.; Eitel, A. Signals Foster Multimedia Learning by Supporting Integration of Highlighted Text and Diagram Elements. Learn. Instr. 2015, 36, 11–26.
  13. de Koning, B.B.; Tabbers, H.K.; Rikers, R.M.J.P.; Paas, F. Towards a Framework for Attention Cueing in Instructional Animations: Guidelines for Research and Design. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 21, 113–140.
  14. Kercood, S.; Zentall, S.S.; Vinh, M.; Tom-Wright, K. Attentional Cuing in Math Word Problems for Girls At-Risk for ADHD and Their Peers in General Education Settings. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2012, 37, 106–112.
  15. Hyönä, J.; Niemi, P. Eye Movements during Repeated Reading of a Text. Acta Psychol. Amst. 1990, 73, 259–280.
  16. Just, M.A.; Carpenter, P.A. A Theory of Reading: From Eye Fixations to Comprehension. Psychol. Rev. 1980, 87, 329–354.
  17. Verschaffel, L.; De Corte, E.; Pauwels, A. Solving Compare Problems: An Eye Movement Test of Lewis and Mayer’s Consistency Hypothesis. J. Educ. Psychol. 1992, 84, 85–94.
  18. van Gog, T.; Scheiter, K. Eye Tracking as a Tool to Study and Enhance Multimedia Learning. Learn. Instr. 2010, 20, 95–99.
  19. Bednarik, R.; Eivazi, S.; Vrzakova, H. A Computational Approach for Prediction of Problem-Solving Behavior Using Support Vector Machines and Eye-Tracking Data. In Eye Gaze in Intelligent User Interfaces; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013; pp. 111–134.
  20. Brunyé, T.T.; Taylor, H.A. When Goals Constrain: Eye Movements and Memory for Goal-Oriented Map Study. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2009, 23, 772–787.
  21. Schwonke, R.; Berthold, K.; Renkl, A. How Multiple External Representations Are Used and How They Can Be Made More Useful. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2009, 23, 1227–1243.
  22. Susac, A.; Bubic, A.; Kaponja, J.; Planinic, M.; Palmovic, M. Eye Movements Reveal Students’ Strategies in Simple Equation Solving. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2014, 12, 555–577.
  23. She, H.-C.; Chen, Y.-Z. The Impact of Multimedia Effect on Science Learning: Evidence from Eye Movements. Comput. Educ. 2009, 53, 1297–1307.
  24. Wu, C.J.; Liu, C.Y.; Yang, C.H.; Jian, Y.C. Eye-Movements Reveal Children’s Deliberative Thinking and Predict Performance on Arithmetic Word Problems. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2021, 36, 91–108.
More
Information
Contributors MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register : , , ,
View Times: 106
Revisions: 2 times (View History)
Update Date: 25 Dec 2023
1000/1000