Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 + 858 word(s) 858 2021-06-16 07:44:56 |
2 Format correct Meta information modification 858 2021-06-16 10:03:51 | |
3 Changed the cover Meta information modification 858 2021-06-16 10:05:31 |

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
El-Kishawi, M.; Khalaf, K. Motor Skill Learning. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/10897 (accessed on 26 April 2024).
El-Kishawi M, Khalaf K. Motor Skill Learning. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/10897. Accessed April 26, 2024.
El-Kishawi, Mohamed, Khaled Khalaf. "Motor Skill Learning" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/10897 (accessed April 26, 2024).
El-Kishawi, M., & Khalaf, K. (2021, June 16). Motor Skill Learning. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/10897
El-Kishawi, Mohamed and Khaled Khalaf. "Motor Skill Learning." Encyclopedia. Web. 16 June, 2021.
Motor Skill Learning
Edit

Motor skill learning requires the control and integration of a range of stimuli and responses to be able to perform the desired motor task. How can we explain, support, or predict how people learn these skills? Several learning theories have been developed to explain how learning motor skills occurs and what stimulates individuals to learn and change.

motor skills learning theories dentistry self-consciousness working memory

1. Introduction

Many contemporary theories of motor skill learning identified the importance of cognitive processes during motor skill acquisition, particularly in the initial stages of learning. The initial stage of motor skill learning (i.e., cognitive stage/declarative stage) involves cognitive processing of verbal/visual instructions related to the task and rehearsal of the task in working memory. This cognitive processing facilitates the interpretation of the instructions required to perform the task.

2. Understanding Motor Skill Learning as Related to Dentistry

Procedural and cognitive skills are essential abilities for clinical dental practice. Students learn these skills during simulated clinical activities designed to ensure that they achieve a satisfactory level prior to proceeding to direct patient care. These simulated activities have associated high costs in terms of staffing and facilities [1][2][3]. To optimize learning in these settings, the design of relevant learning activities needs to be informed by theory and based on evidence.
However, there has only been limited research conducted in relation to the design of the most effective and efficient methods for learning the complex cognitive and fine motor skills required for patient care in dentistry [1][4][5][6]. Similarly, there are few publications discussing the rationale and design of simulation and clinical endodontic learning activities [7][8][9].
Other than investigation of the design and diameter of hand file handles and the effect of the fit of gloves on performance [10][11][12], there has been limited use of learning theories to explicitly inform the design of simulation and clinical dental learning activities [6][13][14][15].
In dentistry, understanding relevant learning theories is essential for dental educators to be able to design effective learning activities, with a clear rationale that supports their dental students’ learning. Below, we will discuss five key theories that have relevance to learning procedural skills and declarative knowledge in dentistry. Specifically, these are Schema theory, Cognitive Load theory, OPTIMAL theory of motor learning, the Novice-Expert continuum and deliberate practice principles, and Reinvestment theory (Table 2).
Table 2. Summary of motor learning theories relevant to dentistry.
Theory Description Points in Favor Points Against
1. Schema Theory [16] Motor learning involves ongoing processes that update the recall and recognition of proprioceptive information from limbs and fingers. The response parameters (e.g., speed and force) are specified according to stored knowledge of the results.
-
Commonly used theory.
-
Used to explain procedural skill development.
-
No longer valid for understanding motor skill learning.
-
Unable to describe learning through observation.
-
Unable to explain the role of augmented feedback.
2. The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning [17] Focuses on discovering the correct instructional approach to support motivation and direction of motor learning to the desired outcome of the motor task.
-
Supports simplifying movement instructions.
-
Positive impact on instructional design.
-
Reduces the load on the working memory.
-
Limited evidence addressing complex fine motor skill learning.
3. Cognitive Load Theory [18] Based on the assumption that cognitive system is limited as working memory can only store and process a small amount of information for a few seconds.
-
Knowledge build up by combining simple elements can result in development of more complex results.
-
Reduces the load on working memory.
-
Positive impact on instructional design.
-
Lacks conceptual clarity and validity of instrument used.
-
Lack generalizability in different contexts.
-
Uses self-reported survey to measure cognitive loads.
4. Novice-Expert continuum and deliberate practice principles [19] Development of expert motor performance depends on continuous deliberate practice improved by trial-and-error learning and supported by appropriate supervision.
-
Supports gradual buildup and improvement of motor skills.
-
Supports safe and low risk buildup of competency.
-
Does not address individual’s cognitive, attentional and perceptual abilities.
-
Requires a long deliberate practice to achieve expert level.
5. Reinvestment Theory [20] Based on the distinction between individual’s movement self-consciousness features related to movement processing and decision making.
-
Commonly used theory.
-
Implicit learning reduces the load on the working memory.
-
Supports simplifying movement instructions.
-
Implicit learning maintains robust performance under multi-tasking and stressful conditions.
-
Limited evidence addressing complex fine motor skill learning.
-
Lack of consensus related to the role of observational learning as an implicit learning approach.
-
Uses self-reported survey to measure the level of reinvestment.

3. Conclusions

There is limited evidence regarding fine motor skill learning in dentistry, particularly in endodontics. It has been shown that learning implicitly when carrying out a motor skill may limit the effect of self-focus and self-regulation on subsequent performance.
Stressors related to dental students’ transferring from simulation to clinical settings can result in deterioration of performance. It is suggested that learning implicitly in the simulation stage can reduce disruptions in performance when moving to clinical settings. Therefore, further investigation of effective methods for learning fine dental motor skills is indicated, using conditions that result in robust performance, even under multi-tasking or stressful conditions.

References

  1. Tedesco, L. Issues in dental curriculum development and change. J. Dent. Educ. 1995, 59, 97–147.
  2. Glickman, G.; Gluskin, A.; Johnson, W.; Lin, J. The crisis in endodontic education: Current perspectives and strategies for change. J. Endod. 2005, 31, 255–261.
  3. McNally, M.; Dunning, D.; Lange, B.; Gound, T. A survey of endodontic residents’ attitudes about a career in dental education. J. Endod. 2002, 28, 592–594.
  4. Knight, G.; Guenzel, P.; Feil, P. Using questions to facilitate motor skill acquisition. J. Dent. Educ. 1997, 61, 56–65.
  5. Wierinck, E.; Puttemans, V.; Swinnen, S.; van Steenberghe, D. Expert performance on a virtual reality simulation system. J. Dent. Educ. 2007, 71, 759–766.
  6. Winning, T.; Malhotra, N.; Masters, R. Investigating an errorless learning approach for developing dental operative technique skills: A pilot study. Eur. J. Dent. Educ. 2018, 22, e706–e714.
  7. Koedijker, J.; Poolton, J.; Maxwell, J.; Oudejans, R.; Beek, P.; Masters, R. Attention and time constraints in perceptual-motor learning and performance: Instruction, analogy, and skill level. Conscious. Cogn. 2011, 20, 245–256.
  8. Friedlander, L.; Anderson, V. A new predoctoral endodontic module: Evaluating learning and effectiveness. J. Dent. Educ. 2011, 75, 351–359.
  9. Sonntag, D.; Barwald, R.; Hulsmann, M.; Stachniss, V. Pre-clinical endodontics: A survey amongst German dental schools. Int. Endod. J. 2008, 41, 863–868.
  10. Chandler, N.; Shaw, J.; Treble, S. Effect of endodontic instrument handle diameter on operator performance. J. Endod. 1996, 22, 110–111.
  11. Chandler, N.; Ford, T.; Monteith, B. Pulp size in molars: Underestimation on radiographs. J. Oral. Rehabil. 2004, 31, 764–769.
  12. Min, L.; Yun-hui, L.; Qiang, H. An optimized haptic interaction model based on support vector regression for evaluation of endodontic shaping skill. In Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Biomimetics (ROBIO), Sanya, China, 15–18 December 2007; pp. 617–622.
  13. El-Kishawi, M.; Khalaf, K.; Masters, R.; Winning, T. Effect of errorless learning on the acquisition of fine motor skills in pre-clinical endodontics. Aust. Endod. J. 2021, 47, 43–53.
  14. El-Kishawi, M.Y.; Khalaf, K.; Odeh, R.M. Determining the impact of stressors on students’ clinical performance in endodontics. J. Taibah. Univ. Med. Sci. 2021.
  15. El-Kishawi, M.; Khalaf, K.; Winning, T. How to Improve Fine Motor Skill Learning in Dentistry. Int. J. Dent. 2021, 2021, 1–8.
  16. Schmidt, R. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychol. Rev. 1975, 82, 225–260.
  17. Wulf, G.; Lewthwaite, R. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016, 23, 1382–1414.
  18. Sweller, J. Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cogn. Sci. 1988, 12, 257–285.
  19. Dreyfus, H.; Dreyfus, S.; Zadeh, L. Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. IEEE Expert 1987, 2, 110–111.
  20. Masters, R.; Polman, R.; Hammond, N. ‘Reinvestment’: A dimension of personality implicated in skill breakdown under pressure. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1993, 14, 655–666.
More
Information
Contributors MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register : ,
View Times: 612
Revisions: 3 times (View History)
Update Date: 16 Jun 2021
1000/1000