Languages, Dialects and Educational Achievement: History
Please note this is an old version of this entry, which may differ significantly from the current revision.
Contributor:

This item focuses on the development of usage of Standard Dutch versus regional languages and dialects. It also explores the relation between language choice and language and mathematics test performance of young children. Data were collected in 2014 and the sample analyzed included 3,639 grade 2 pupils from 437 Dutch primary schools. The results of the present analyses were compared with those from analyses performed on similar data collected twenty years earlier, in 1994. Findings show that there has been dramatic decline in usage of regional languages and dialects. No correlation was found between language choice and language and mathematic test performance.

 

  • regional languages
  • dialects
  • standard language
  • Dutch
  • Frisian
  • Limburgish
  • the Netherlands
  • language achievement
  • language variety
  • replication study
  • language development
  • language loss

1. Regional languages and dialects in decline 

In the Netherlands, the standard language is Standard Dutch, but there are also three regional languages which are recognized by the European Charter for Regional or Minority Languages (Frisian, Limburgish, and Low Saxon, which are spoken in parts of the provinces of Friesland; Limburg; and Groningen, Drenthe, Overijssel and Gelderland, respectively), and in addition to this there are many local dialects. Since a number of decades, the usage of regional languages and dialects is in severe decline. In a large-scale study (Driessen & Withagen, 1999), which analyzed data collected in 1994 from 7730 primary school pupils and their parents it was found that: 13% of the children spoke a regional language or dialect, while 26% of their parents did so; large differences in language choice exist between provinces; higher educated parents have a better command of Dutch than lower educated parents; the correlation of the children’s language and mathematics achievement and their language choice is weak at best.

2. Research questions and design 

The present study (Driessen, 2016) is a replication of the 1994 study, i.e. comparable data were analyzed with the same variables and computations. There were two main questions: (1) How often do young children and their parents speak a regional language or dialect in 2014 and does this differ from the situation in 1994? (2) Is there any correlation between the children’s language choice (i.e. speaking a regional language or dialect versus Standard Dutch) and their academic language and math achievement?

The data are from the 2014 wave of the national large-scale COOL5-18 cohort study. A total of 437 primary schools participated with 3639 native-born pupils in second grade (6-year-olds) and their parents. (Immigrant pupils and their parents were excluded.)

The pupils’ parents completed a questionnaire with questions related to, among other things, social background and language choice patterns and language proficiency.

From the child’s perspective, four questions were related to the language they generally spoke in conversations in four domains, with: (a) mother; (b) father; (c) siblings; (d) friends. The parents were also asked which language they generally spoke with their partner. In both cases the relevant answer categories were: (1) Dutch; (2) a regional language or dialect. For the children a sum score was computed relative to the number of domains they spoke Dutch, ranging from 0% (in none of the four domains) to 100% (in all of the four domains).

Each of the parents were asked about their command of the Dutch language, distinguishing four language modalities: (a) understand; (b) speak; (c) read; (d) write. The ultimate answer categories were: (1) poor; (2) fairly good; (3) very good.

The parents also provided information regarding their highest level of education.

Two multiple choice achievement tests were administered to the children, language and mathematics. The results were expressed as so-called skill scores.

3.  Results

The first two columns of Table 1 show the relative number of times the children speak Dutch in the four domains discerned for 1994, and twenty years later in 2014. For the Netherlands as a whole, in 1994, the children generally spoke Dutch in 87% of the domains, while in 2014 this had increased to 93%. This means a 6% point loss of regional language or dialect speaking. Columns three and four show that for the parents this loss is even more dramatic, viz. 17% points. There are significant differences according to province, or, perhaps better, regional language. Especially in Limburg and Friesland there still are relative many children and – even more – parents who speak their regional language.

Table 1 - Language mostly spoken by child is Dutch and language mostly spoken by parents is Dutch, by year and province (mean percentages)

Province

Language child Dutch

Language parents Dutch

 

1994

2014

1994

2014

Groningen (3)

88

93

58

78

Friesland (1)

53

68

42

65

Drenthe (3)

81

93

44

78

Overijssel (3)

92

97

64

90

Flevoland

99

86

91

89

Gelderland (3)

97

96

78

95

Utrecht

100

97

97

100

North-Holland

97

97

96

99

South-Holland

99

97

98

98

Zeeland

85

92

69

83

North-Brabant

96

97

81

99

Limburg (2)

53

68

39

50

The Netherlands

87

93

74

91

(1) Frisian; (2) Low Saxon; (3) Limburgish

 

Table 2 shows the correlation between the children’s language and mathematics test scores and a number of background characteristics. For this analysis of variance the background characteristics have been collapsed into a number of (ordinal) classes. What is important here is the eta statistic, which can range from 0.00 to 1.00 (and which is comparable to the Pearson r correlation statistic).

 

Table 2 – Language and mathematics proficiency and background characteristics (mean scores)

 

 

Language

Math

 

Language

Math

Language child Dutch

 

 

Proficiency Dutch parents

 

 

0 times

67.7

86.1

very bad – good

64.3

82.5

1-3 times

67.7

85.2

good – very good

66.6

84.3

4 times

67.8

85.9

very good

68.5

86.6

eta

0.00

0.01

eta

0.10*

0.10*

Language parents

 

 

Sex

 

 

Dutch

67.9

85.9

boy

66.7

85.4

regional language, dialect

67.5

85.6

girl

68.9

86.1

eta

0.00

0.00

eta

0.10*

0.03

Education parents

 

 

Region

 

 

jun. vocational

63.2

81.1

Frisian

68.8

84.8

jun. general

65.7

83.8

Low Saxon

67.6

85.1

sen. vocational

66.6

84.1

Limburgish

68.3

87.5

sen. general

67.0

86.2

Brabants

68.1

86.8

higher professional

68.8

86.8

Zeeuws

68.3

85.2

university

70.8

89.6

Hollandic

67.5

85.5

eta

0.18*

0.19*

eta

0.04

0.07*

* p<0.01

 

The data in Table 2 make it clear that for the children’s language and mathematics proficiency it does not matter whether they themselves or their parents speak Dutch or a regional language or dialect. Of the background characteristics analyzed here the most important is the level of education of the parents, although it should be mentioned that a correlation of eta = 0.18 still is not very strong.

To answer the two research questions, (1) the data show that the usage of regional languages and dialects indeed is in severe decline, but that there are differences between provinces, and (2) that children who generally speak a regional language or dialect or who have parents who generally speak these language varieties do not perform worse on a Dutch language and mathematics test than children who generally speak Standard Dutch or who have parents who speak Standard Dutch. 

Note

This item is based on: Driessen, G. (2016) Taalvariatie en onderwijsprestaties van autochtone kleuters. Een replicatiestudie na twee decennia. Taal en Tongval, 68(1), 1-12.

References

  1. Driessen, G. (2005). In Dutch? Usage of Dutch regional languages and dialects. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 18(3), 271-285.
  2. Driessen, G. (2006). Ontwikkelingen in het gebruik van streektalen en dialecten in de periode 1995-2003. Toegepaste Taalwetenschap in Artikelen, 75(1), 103-113, 118.
  3. Driessen, G. (2016). Taalvariatie en onderwijsprestaties van autochtone kleuters. Een replicatiestudie na twee decennia. Taal en Tongval, 68(1), 1-12.
  4. Driessen, G. (2016). Zoals de ouden zongen, piepen de jongen? De taal van ouders en taal en taalvaardigheid van hun kinderen. Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5(2), 145-159.
  5. Driessen, G., & Withagen, V. (1999). Language varieties and educational achievement of indigenous primary school pupils. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 12(1), 1-22.
More
This entry is offline, you can click here to edit this entry!
ScholarVision Creations