The COVID-19 pandemic has caused unprecedented changes in the educational system, requiring students to continually switch between distance and in-person learning conditions. Considering the close relationship that has always linked anxiety to mathematics, the present study explores the differences in the anxiety levels of students towards mathematics during distance or in-person school learning. During the second wave of COVID-19, 405 students, recruited from twelve middle schools of Catania province (Italy), completed an online version of the MeMa questionnaire, answering each item twice and imagining themselves to be, respectively, in distance and in-person learning conditions. The results showed a minor state of anxiety experienced during distance learning. However, the students who preferred to learn mathematics in person revealed less mathematics anxiety and better mental states and metacognitive awareness; the same results were found in those who reported higher math marks and who preferred scientific subjects. It seems that math anxiety is not one of the various flaws that are imputed to distance learning. Our findings encourage a reflection on possible interventions to reduce students’ anxiety by working on motivation and dysfunctional beliefs.
1. Introduction
For two years now, the COVID-19 infection has changed the way students experience school. Indeed, as is generally known, their habits have been upset by shifting many times from in-person learning (PL) to distance learning (DL) conditions to manage and reduce the risk of contagion. Focusing on the Italian context, since March 2020 pupils have been experiencing the advantages and drawbacks of e-learning platforms, whose global market had already surpassed 200 billion in 2019 and was expected to have a compound annual growth rate of over 8% between 2020–2026
[1]. Although it is clear that historical events related to the COVID-19 pandemic have determined a major development of this technological sector, the cognitive, emotional, and social implications of distance, blended, and in-person learning conditions still need to be fully explored.
Sabirova et al.
[2], for instance, wondered how those changes influenced pupils’ academic stress and school wellbeing, defined, respectively, as the whole spectrum of negative physical and emotional states associated with the educational process and as a multidimensional construct comprising emotional and cognitive components connected to the scholastic environment
[3]. This important question needs to be framed within a general worsening of pupils’ mental health: according to many authors, during the COVID-19 pandemic more than one-fifth of junior high and high school students experienced negative psychological effects, such as severe levels of anxiety, depression, and stress
[4][5][6]. Moreover, as pointed out by Bignardi et al.
[7], who cited a longitudinal study of 13–14-year-old pupils in the UK, anxiety and depression increased after the first COVID-19 wave and decreased slightly during the second one, but the pupils have not fully recovered yet. Lanius et al.
[8], in a study that explored undergraduate students’ experience with the emergency transition to remote learning and measured their math anxiety before and after the transition, revealed that “factors that directly impacted a student’s learning experience with a high impact on changes in math anxiety include communication with the instructor as well as technology quality and access” (p. 168). Due to the transition to online education, which involved much more time spent on e-platforms, with a consequent loss of social interactions, lockdown-related mental health complaints were directly associated with distance learning
[9].
2. Students’ Mathematics Anxiety
Mathematical skills are an essential ability for life, and they allow better outcomes in studies and job careers
[10]. However, when compared to other subjects, one of the most frequently reported emotions linked to mathematics is anxiety, which increases with age and hinders math skills, causing avoidance and low mastery of mathematical abilities
[11]. MA is defined as the apprehension that one has about the capacity to do mathematics or ‘an illogical feeling of panic, embarrassment, flurry, avoidance, failing and fear, which are physically visible, and which prevent solution, learning and success about mathematics’
[12] (p. 312). It is linked to several factors: the influence of teachers, the influence of parents, the impact of the teaching method, the perception of math difficulty, the pressure of time limits on tests, the fear of public embarrassment, and the belief that math performances are a measure of intelligence
[13]. It has been documented that MA produces its effects on physiological, cognitive, and emotional levels. Higher-math-anxious students reported increased heart rates
[14], clammy hands, and when they face an upcoming mathematical task, they show neural activations similar to those found when individuals experience physical pain
[15]. MA impairs the proper functioning of working memory, which is crucial as it is involved in more complex calculations such as multistep mathematical problems
[16][17]. Students with MA describe feelings of nervousness, apprehension, and worry
[18]. It is clear, therefore, that MA is not related to cognitive difficulties only, but also to affective factors and beliefs
[19]. This calls for a specific attention towards the mental states associated with mathematics, encompassing the beliefs on personal mathematical skills and the motivation and attitudes towards the subject
[20].
There is a negative relationship between MA and math performance
[21][22]. Moreover, the literature shows higher levels of mathematics anxiety in females than in males
[23][24] and in higher levels of education
[25][26][27]. Moreover, metacognition seems to moderate math anxiety and predicts that performance will decrease as anxiety increases, except at high metacognition levels; furthermore, metacognition predicts confidence in accuracy: people with higher metacognition are more confident in their ability to answer problems correctly
[28].
oreover, according to Lukowski et al.
[29], MA should be approached as a multidimensional construct as it encompasses anxiety in performing mathematical calculations, anxiety about math in classroom situations, and anxiety about math tests. In other words, tension in both learning and doing mathematics and in being evaluated on mathematical skills are included.
Considering that which is mentioned above, our research interest focused on how, with regard to mathematics, anxiety, mental states—which are, as stated before, the beliefs about personal mathematical skills and the motivation and attitudes towards the subject—and metacognitive awareness, which is understood as the personal knowledge on the mathematical learning process, vary in distance and in-person learning settings. The impact of the two conditions on metacognition, beliefs, and anxiety has not been explored yet, considering the exceptionality of the COVID-19 emergency, even if some empirical data regarding the differences between online and face-to-face schooling have been published recently
[30].
3. The present study
We used a prospective approach to build up our research design. Thanks to the literature, we pointed out the significant increase in psychological distress in students during this long COVID-19 pandemic
[31][32], and the different ways of schooling they have been experiencing for the first time in their scholastic history. As a consequence, we selected our general goal and the constructs relevant for the research: we intended to explore the differences in anxiety, mental states, and metacognitive awareness towards mathematics in both distance and in-person learning conditions among middle school students in Italy. With regard to our methodological approach, keeping in mind our main objective, we assumed PL and DL as independent variables and all the variables belonging to our online questionnaire (see below for further information) as our dependent variables. Considering the exceptionality of the situation, our main interrogatives were: as teaching conditions vary, DL or PL, did math anxiety and its correlates change? Did having high or low marks relate to the preference towards DL or PL? Our objective was threefold: (1) we wanted to understand whether students experience different levels of MA (distinguished by learning, evaluation, and general anxiety), mental states, and metacognitive awareness in distance vs. in-person learning conditions; (2) we wanted to verify the existence of differences between those who prefer the DL or PL of mathematics in terms of MA, mental states, metacognitive awareness, math marks, and favorite subject; and (3) we wanted to evaluate gender differences for all the variables considered in this study. The final goal, in conclusion, was to understand whether MA is related to learning conditions and whether other variables should be taken into account.
3.1. Participants
Even though N = 462 participants were enrolled in the online survey, N = 57 questionnaires were unfinished and consequently removed from the final dataset. A total of N = 405 Italian students, 222 females and 183 males, aged between 11 and 14 years (M = 12.56; SD = 0.64) took part in the study.
At the beginning of the study, we contacted about 25 middle schools in Catania and its province (Italy), but only 12 decided to participate in our research. In the Italian schooling system, middle schools last three years and are attended after the first level of education (i.e., primary school) from the ages of 11 to 14. Prior to the beginning of the study, ethical approval was granted from the first author’s university ethics committee. The study obtained ethical permission from the Department of Educational Sciences Catania University Internal Ethics Review Board for psychological research (16 June 2020).
3.2. Procedure
After the selection of the psychometric tool to administer, the questions were transferred to the Google Form platform and sent to many school principals. Twelve of them accepted the invitation to participate in the research. The form, therefore, was sent to math teachers who, in turn, sent it via email or WhatsApp to the students. They completed it at home in the afternoon.
All the questions were mandatory, and their completion lasted about 10 min. The administration took place during the COVID-19 pandemic, and the pupils had already experienced at least 3/4 months of distance learning, divided into different times, depending on the trend of the infections and the closure of schools.
Each participant received a full description of the scope and the protocol of the study. The confidentiality of the responses was also assured. Before starting the protocol, a consent form was signed by their parents. Institutional approval was granted.
4. Findings
Most of the participants prefer in-person learning (71.6%) rather than distance learning (28.4%). This pattern becomes more evident when the students choose between the PL and DL of mathematics. In fact, a higher percentage of participants (85.4%) prefer in-person math classes rather than distance math classes (14.6%).
4.1. Differences between DL and PL of Math on Anxiety, Mental States, and Metacognitive Awareness
To begin, we performed a paired t test to understand in which learning condition students experience more math anxiety, as well as better mental states and higher metacognitive awareness.
The students participating in our study reveal a higher in-person math learning and evaluation anxiety, which is a major tension induced, respectively, by the teacher’s explanations and by having to perform difficult tasks; they also show a higher generalized school anxiety in PL; that is, anxiety is extended to all other disciplines, not just math. Moreover, our sample refers to having a higher metacognitive awareness in mathematics, namely the beliefs that students have about mathematics skills, discipline, and learning, during DL. There are no significant differences regarding mental states.
4.2. Gender Differences on Anxiety, Considered Both in DL and PL
Considering these first results, and considering the novelty of this study, we wanted to verify whether there were gender differences. Females experience higher levels of math evaluation anxiety both in person (females M = 21.85, SD = 6.73; males M = 18.79, SD = 6.69; t =−4.55, p = 0.002, d = −0.4) and online (females M = 20.33, SD = 6.40; males M = 17.26, SD = 6.47; t = −4.77, p = 0.002, d = −0.4). Gender also affects generalized school anxiety with females reporting higher scores both in person (females M = 12.86, SD = 4.10; males M = 11.28, SD = 3.70; t = −4.00, p = 0.002, d = −0.4) and online (females M = 11.90, SD = 3.91; males M = 10.33, SD = 3.50; t = −4.21, p = 0.002, d = −0.4).
4.3. Differences between Students Who Prefer Math by Distance Learning or Math by In-Person Learning
Those who prefer to learn math in person obtained better scores on mental states, metacognitive awareness, and math learning anxiety, both when mathematics is learnt in person and when it is learnt by distance learning.
4.4. Students’ Math Marks and MeMa Variables
There is a relationship between math marks and the expressed preference towards the DL and PL of mathematics, as well as between math marks and the MeMa results. Indeed, the participants who prefer the PL of math have better grades in math (M = 7.48, SD = 1.48) than the group that prefers DL (M = 6.54, SD = 1.43), with t = −4.53 and p = 0.001. Furthermore, Pearson correlations indicate a negative correlation between math marks and the three MeMa anxiety subscales, both in PL and DL (PL math learning anxiety r =−0.364; p = < 0.001; DL math learning anxiety r = −0.34; p = 0.001; PL math evaluation anxiety r = −0.23; p = < 0.001; DL math evaluation anxiety r = −0.23; p = < 0.001; PL generalized school anxiety r = −0.16; p = < 0.001; DL generalized school anxiety r = −0.14; p = < 0.001). Moreover, we observed a positive correlation between math marks and mental states and metacognitive awareness both in DL and PL (PL mental states r = 0.52; p = 0.001; DL mental states r = 0.49; p = 0.001; PL metacognitive awareness r = 0.34; p = 0.001; DL metacognitive awareness r = 0.37; p = 0.001).
4.5. Differences in Anxiety between Students Who Chose Their Favorite Subject
Considering those last results, we also chose to explore the differences in MeMa anxiety between students who expressed a preference for a certain subject (grouped into Humanities, Science, Languages, and Art subjects). Students who prefer scientific subjects present significantly lower levels of anxiety in all its three dimensions, both in the DL and the PL of math.
5. Conclusion
The first result obtained from this study could have easily misled us: although the students prefer to attend school as well as learn mathematics in PL, they experience lower levels of anxiety in math in DL rather than in PL. However, an in-depth investigation prompted us to observe the data from a different perspective, which highlighted that preferring mathematics and having good marks could decrease the importance attributed to the fact that math is taught in DL or PL. The preference for mathematics, which describes those who like it, plays an important role. On the one hand, if students perceive themselves to be good and well-evaluated, the way in which mathematics is taught does not matter, the anxiety will be lower because at the root there is the fact of loving mathematics. On the other hand, it is also true that lower levels of anxiety could facilitate the development of a better mathematics self-esteem and a preference for the subject. Further studies could investigate the causal direction of this relationship.
However, according to our results, the recent development of learning technologies, boosted by public health concerns, should not be the only target for interventions focused on the emotional connotations of mathematics, whose reputation is one of being an anxiogenic subject. According to our work, MA needs, rather, to be addressed by interventions focused on the attitudes of students and on their beliefs, metacognition, and motivation. To the best of our knowledge, this is one of the first studies investigating the subject-related emotions between different conditions of learning; so, few materials for comparisons could be found. We look forward to more development of the studies on the topic so that specific schooling strategies can be defined and applied for the wellbeing of students.
This entry is adapted from the peer-reviewed paper 10.3390/educsci12060379