Intracranial Neurofeedback System on Memory Function: History
Please note this is an old version of this entry, which may differ significantly from the current revision.
Contributor: , , , , , ,

Neurofeedback (NF) shows promise in enhancing memory, but its application to the medial temporal lobe (MTL) still needs to be studied. Twenty trials of a tug-of-war game per session were employed for NF and designed to control neural activity bidirectionally (Up/Down condition).

  • neurofeedback
  • memory enhancement
  • medial temporal lobe
  • intracranial electrode
  • bidirectional control
  • memory encoding
  • intracranial electroencephalogram
  • epilepsy

1. Introduction

Several studies targeting patients with lesions in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) have reported impairments in short-term memory function [1][2][3][4][5]. Recent research in rats indicates that the coordinated neural activity patterns between the dentate gyrus and the hippocampal CA3 region contribute to working memory [6]. Additionally, it was reported that damage to the right MTL may lead to a heightened propensity for impairments in spatial memory, integral to navigation and self-location, while damage to the left MTL may increase deficits in verbal memory, essential for memorising auditory and visual linguistic information [7].
In recent years, electrical stimulation to deep brain regions has been established, leading to an increase in studies aiming to enhance memory targeting the MTL [8]. However, some studies specifically targeting the hippocampus have reported a decline in memory [9][10][11][12]. This decline is hypothesised to result from an acute depolarization block leading to memory impairment [9]. Akin to electrical stimulation, neurofeedback (NF) is one of the techniques for modulating brain function. NF converts brain activity into perceptible information, such as bar length or circle size, and provides feedback, allowing individuals to self-regulate their brain activity. Through repeated NF training, it is expected that the regulation is facilitated of cognitive functions associated with brain activity. NF has gained momentum in clinical applications, with reports of symptom alleviation in various mental disorders, such as anxiety, depression, schizophrenia [13], and ADHD [14]. Additionally, there has been an increase in opportunities for NF training in healthy individuals to enhance memory [15], meditation [16], and attentional focus [17]. Since NF relies on the brain’s autonomic learning capability to regulate neural activity, it offers a potentially lower risk of interfering with memory function compared to electrical stimulation. Moreover, anatomical evidence reveals that memory function involves not only the hippocampus but also multiple regions, such as the thalamus, mammillary bodies, and the cingulate gyrus [18].
Patients with drug-resistant epilepsy, undergoing intracranial electrode placement through craniotomy for the purpose of epileptic focus diagnosis, provide an opportunity for intracranial electroencephalogram (iEEG) measurements. Recently, there has been increased interest in applying this invasive brain activity measurement method to brain-computer interfaces [19]. Signals obtained from intracranial electrodes offer superior temporal resolution compared to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and reduce the impact of artefacts compared to scalp EEG. Additionally, the signals are not attenuated by the dura mater, skull, or scalp tissue. These advantages can be beneficial in NF studies, such as those focusing on modulating somatosensory–motor function [20][21][22] or emotional function [23]. However, these approaches are still in their early stages, and there are few NF studies specifically targeting memory function in the MTL [24].

2. Learning Mechanisms in Neurofeedback

The mechanism behind NF training is believed to be rooted in operant conditioning [25]. Operant conditioning involves changing the occurrence probability of a response under a specific condition by providing a consequence (reinforcement or punishment) in response to voluntary behaviour (operant behaviour). The “specific condition” serves as an antecedent that functions as a cue for the voluntary response. Operant conditioning involves learning the association, known as the three-term contingency, between antecedent, behaviour, and consequence.
In the context of NF, considering an illustrative scenario where the user can increase the length of a bar displayed on the screen when a specific brain activity pattern occurs during a specific cognitive condition. Through NF training, as the user engages in trial and error, they may accidentally produce the desired brain activity pattern and achieve success in increasing the length of the bar. Through repeated these experiences, the user’s probability of generating the desired brain activity pattern in a specific cognitive condition gradually increases. In other words, in NF, the user learns the contingency between the specific cognitive condition (antecedent), the brain activity pattern (behaviour), and the success in bar length adjustment (reinforcement).
The presence of consequence is crucial for the success of operant conditioning. If the reinforcements or punishments following operant behaviour are insufficient, the changes in the occurrence probability of the response will be weak, making it difficult for learning to take place. A neurophysiological theory of NF suggests the significant contribution of the striatum, a part of the reward system, to NF [26], and indeed, several NF studies have reported the observed involvement of the striatum [27][28][29]. These emphasise the importance of the consequence in NF as well.

3. Challenges in Neurofeedback Research on Memory Function

3.1. Challenges in Brain Activity Measurement

NF studies targeting memory have extensively used scalp EEG [15][30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37]. Memory function has long been suggested to be associated with theta oscillations [38][39][40], and an increase in theta power during memory encoding has been reported in many EEG studies [38][41][42]. Based on the accumulation of such foundational research, many memory EEGNF studies have been implemented for increasing theta power. So far, previous studies have shown that upregulation of theta activity in the frontal midline improves control processes during memory retrieval [31] and accelerates and enhances memory consolidation [33][36]. Studies utilising intracranial electrodes have also reported increased theta power in the MTL during successful encoding and retrieval [43][44][45][46][47][48][49], and increased theta phase synchronization between the hippocampus and other cortical regions (rhinal cortex, prefrontal cortex) during memory encoding [50][51]. However, some studies also report a decrease in MTL theta power during encoding [52][53][54][55][56][57][58], and the causal relationship between MTL theta power changes (increases or decreases) and memory function (encoding success or failure) is inconsistent across studies.

3.2. Challenges in Modulating Memory Function

In NF studies aiming to improve mental disorders, it is common to conduct NF during a resting state without imposing additional tasks on participants. This is because the resting state already corresponds to the “specific cognitive condition” in the learning mechanism of NF. In other words, in mental disorders, the chronic symptoms typically persist even during the resting state, which is why NF in this state enables the learning of three-term contingency. The same applies to healthy individuals training their attention, relaxation, or meditation using NF. On the other hand, when conducting NF training for memory function, the “specific cognitive condition” corresponds to the condition requiring memory function. Therefore, it is essential to create a situation that necessitates memory function, such as imposing memory tasks on participants, and conduct NF within that context to efficiently learn the three-term contingency and achieve successful memory NF. Traditional memory NF studies have often required several weeks or more for participants to gain self-regulation of brain activity [15][59]. This may be attributed to the fact that they were conducted during resting states, without creating a situation that necessitates memory function. Additionally, during the resting state, participants lack relevant clues on how to control their brain activity to adjust the feedback signal, leading to higher difficulty in learning self-regulation and requiring an extended time for the effects to appear. If NF can be conducted within a context that necessitates the desired brain function, participants will have clues on how to change the feedback signal, potentially making it easier to acquire self-regulation of brain function in a shorter period.
However, when participants perform tasks requiring a specific brain function while simultaneously receiving feedback, dual-task interference occurs, diverting attention away from the feedback, making self-regulation difficult, and vice versa. To address this issue, a task-based NF approach has been proposed [60][61], where the periods for task performance and feedback are separated, allowing for alternating short periods of task engagement and feedback. This approach enables participants to engage in tasks and feedback without divided attention.

This entry is adapted from the peer-reviewed paper 10.3390/biomedicines11082262

References

  1. Aggleton, J.P.; Shaw, C.; Gaffan, E.A. The Performance of Postencephalitic Amnesic Subjects on Two Behavioural Tests of Memory: Concurrent Discrimination Learning and Delayed Matching-to-Sample. Cortex 1992, 28, 359–372.
  2. Buffalo, E.A.; Reber, P.J.; Squire, L.R. The Human Perirhinal Cortex and Recognition Memory. Hippocampus 1986, 8, 330–339.
  3. Holdstock, J.S.; Shaw, C.; Aggleton, J.P. The Performance of Amnesic Subjects on Tests of Delayed Matching-to-Sample and Delayed Matching-to-Position. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 1583–1596.
  4. Holdstock, J.S.; Gutnikov, S.A.; Gaffan, D.; Mayes, A.R. Perceptual and Mnemonic Matching-to-Sample in Humans: Contributions of the Hippocampus, Perirhinal and Other Medial Temporal Lobe Cortices. Cortex 2000, 36, 301–322.
  5. Owen, A.M.; Sahakian, B.J.; Semple, J.; Polkey, C.E.; Robbins, T.W. Visuo-Spatial Short-Term Recognition Memory and Learning after Temporal Lobe Excisions, Frontal Lobe Excisions or Amygdalo-Hippocampectomy in Man. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 1–24.
  6. Sasaki, T.; Piatti, V.C.; Hwaun, E.; Ahmadi, S.; Lisman, J.E.; Leutgeb, S.; Leutgeb, J.K. Dentate Network Activity Is Necessary for Spatial Working Memory by Supporting CA3 Sharp-Wave Ripple Generation and Prospective Firing of CA3 Neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2018, 21, 258–269.
  7. Bohbot, V.D.; Kalina, M.; Stepankova, K.; Spackova, N.; Petrides, M.; Nadel, L. Spatial Memory Deficits in Patients with Lesions to the Right Hippocampus and to the Right Parahippocampal Cortex. Neuropsychologia 1998, 36, 1217–1238.
  8. Suthana, N.; Haneef, Z.; Stern, J.; Mukamel, R.; Behnke, E.; Knowlton, B.; Fried, I. Memory Enhancement and Deep-Brain Stimulation of the Entorhinal Area. N. Engl. J. Med. 2012, 366, 502–510.
  9. Hescham, S.; Lim, L.W.; Jahanshahi, A.; Blokland, A.; Temel, Y. Deep Brain Stimulation in Dementia-Related Disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2666–2675.
  10. Lacruz, M.E.; Valentín, A.; Seoane, J.J.G.; Morris, R.G.; Selway, R.P.; Alarcón, G. Single Pulse Electrical Stimulation of the Hippocampus Is Sufficient to Impair Human Episodic Memory. Neuroscience 2010, 170, 623–632.
  11. McLachlan, R.S.; Pigott, S.; Tellez-Zenteno, J.F.; Wiebe, S.; Parrent, A. Bilateral Hippocampal Stimulation for Intractable Temporal Lobe Epilepsy: Impact on Seizures and Memory. Epilepsia 2010, 51, 304–307.
  12. Miatton, M.; Van Roost, D.; Thiery, E.; Carrette, E.; Van Dycke, A.; Vonck, K.; Meurs, A.; Vingerhoets, G.; Boon, P. The Cognitive Effects of Amygdalohippocampal Deep Brain Stimulation in Patients with Temporal Lobe Epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2011, 22, 759–764.
  13. Schoenberg, P.L.A.; David, A.S. Biofeedback for Psychiatric Disorders: A Systematic Review. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2014, 39, 109–135.
  14. Lofthouse, N.; Arnold, L.E.; Hersch, S.; Hurt, E.; DeBeus, R. A Review of Neurofeedback Treatment for Pediatric ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2012, 16, 351–372.
  15. Wang, J.R.; Hsieh, S. Neurofeedback Training Improves Attention and Working Memory Performance. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2013, 124, 2406–2420.
  16. Brandmeyer, T.; Delorme, A. Meditation and Neurofeedback. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 688.
  17. Brandmeyer, T.; Delorme, A. Closed-Loop Frontal Midlineθ Neurofeedback: A Novel Approach for Training Focused-Attention Meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 246.
  18. Papez, J.W. A Proposed Mechanism of Emotion. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 1937, 38, 725–743.
  19. Herff, C.; Krusienski, D.J.; Kubben, P. The Potential of Stereotactic-EEG for Brain-Computer Interfaces: Current Progress and Future Directions. Front. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 123.
  20. Gharabaghi, A.; Naros, G.; Khademi, F.; Jesser, J.; Spüler, M.; Walter, A.; Bogdan, M.; Rosenstiel, W.; Birbaumer, N. Learned Self-Regulation of the Lesioned Brain with Epidural Electrocorticography. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 429.
  21. Khanna, P.; Swann, N.C.; De Hemptinne, C.; Miocinovic, S.; Miller, A.; Starr, P.A.; Carmena, J.M. Neurofeedback Control in Parkinsonian Patients Using Electrocorticography Signals Accessed Wirelessly with a Chronic, Fully Implanted Device. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2017, 25, 1715–1724.
  22. Bichsel, O.; Stieglitz, L.H.; Oertel, M.F.; Baumann, C.R.; Gassert, R.; Imbach, L.L. Deep Brain Electrical Neurofeedback Allows Parkinson Patients to Control Pathological Oscillations and Quicken Movements. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 7973.
  23. Yamin, H.G.; Gazit, T.; Tchemodanov, N.; Raz, G.; Jackont, G.; Charles, F.; Fried, I.; Hendler, T.; Cavazza, M. Depth Electrode Neurofeedback with a Virtual Reality Interface. Brain-Comput. Interfaces 2017, 4, 201–213.
  24. Koizumi, K.; Kunii, N.; Ueda, K.; Takabatake, K.; Nagata, K.; Fujitani, S.; Shimada, S.; Nakao, M. Intracranial Neurofeedback Modulating Neural Activity in the Mesial Temporal Lobe during Memory Encoding: A Pilot Study. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2023.
  25. Sherlin, L.H.; Arns, M.; Lubar, J.; Heinrich, H.; Kerson, C.; Strehl, U.; Sterman, M.B. Neurofeedback and Basic Learning Theory: Implications for Research and Practice. J. Neurother. 2011, 15, 292–304.
  26. Davelaar, E.J. Mechanisms of Neurofeedback: A Computation-Theoretic Approach. Neuroscience 2018, 378, 175–188.
  27. Hinterberger, T.; Veit, R.; Wilhelm, B.; Weiskopf, N.; Vatine, J.J.; Birbaumer, N. Neuronal Mechanisms Underlying Control of a Brain–Computer Interface. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005, 21, 3169–3181.
  28. Johnston, S.J.; Boehm, S.G.; Healy, D.; Goebel, R.; Linden, D.E.J. Neurofeedback: A Promising Tool for the Self-Regulation of Emotion Networks. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 1066–1072.
  29. Skottnik, L.; Sorger, B.; Kamp, T.; Linden, D.; Goebel, R. Success and Failure of Controlling the Real-Time Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Neurofeedback Signal Are Reflected in the Striatum. Brain Behav. 2019, 9, e01240.
  30. Enriquez-Geppert, S.; Huster, R.J.; Figge, C.; Herrmann, C.S. Self-Regulation of Frontal-Midline Theta Facilitates Memory Updating and Mental Set Shifting. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 420.
  31. Eschmann, K.C.J.; Bader, R.; Mecklinger, A. Improving Episodic Memory: Frontal-Midline Theta Neurofeedback Training Increases Source Memory Performance. NeuroImage 2020, 222, 117219.
  32. Eschmann, K.C.J.; Mecklinger, A. Improving Cognitive Control: Is Theta Neurofeedback Training Associated with Proactive Rather than Reactive Control Enhancement? Psychophysiology 2022, 59, e13873.
  33. Reiner, M.; Rozengurt, R.; Barnea, A. Better than Sleep: Theta Neurofeedback Training Accelerates Memory Consolidation. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 95, 45–53.
  34. Reiner, M.; Lev, D.D.; Rosen, A. Theta Neurofeedback Effects on Motor Memory Consolidation and Performance Accuracy: An Apparent Paradox? Neuroscience 2018, 378, 198–210.
  35. Reis, J.; Portugal, A.M.; Fernandes, L.; Afonso, N.; Pereira, M.; Sousa, N.; Dias, N.S. An Alpha and Theta Intensive and Short Neurofeedback Protocol for Healthy Aging Working-Memory Training. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 8, 157.
  36. Rozengurt, R.; Shtoots, L.; Sheriff, A.; Sadka, O.; Levy, D.A. Enhancing Early Consolidation of Human Episodic Memory by Theta EEG Neurofeedback. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017, 145, 165–171.
  37. Tseng, Y.H.; Tamura, K.; Okamoto, T. Neurofeedback Training Improves Episodic and Semantic Long-Term Memory Performance. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 17274.
  38. Guderian, S.; Düzel, E. Induced Theta Oscillations Mediate Large-Scale Synchrony with Mediotemporal Areas during Recollection in Humans. Hippocampus 2005, 15, 901–912.
  39. Herweg, N.A.; Apitz, T.; Leicht, G.; Mulert, C.; Fuentemilla, L.; Bunzeck, N. Theta-Alpha Oscillations Bind the Hippocampus, Prefrontal Cortex, and Striatum during Recollection: Evidence from Simultaneous EEG–FMRI. J. Neurosci. 2016, 36, 3579–3587.
  40. Hsieh, L.T.; Ranganath, C. Frontal Midline Theta Oscillations during Working Memory Maintenance and Episodic Encoding and Retrieval. NeuroImage 2014, 85, 721–729.
  41. Hanslmayr, S.; Spitzer, B.; Bäuml, K.H. Brain Oscillations Dissociate between Semantic and Nonsemantic Encoding of Episodic Memories. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 1631–1640.
  42. Osipova, D.; Takashima, A.; Oostenveld, R.; Fernández, G.; Maris, E.; Jensen, O. Theta and Gamma Oscillations Predict Encoding and Retrieval of Declarative Memory. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 7523–7531.
  43. Fell, J.; Ludowig, E.; Staresina, B.P.; Wagner, T.; Kranz, T.; Elger, C.E.; Axmacher, N. Medial Temporal Theta/Alpha Power Enhancement Precedes Successful Memory Encoding: Evidence Based on Intracranial EEG. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 5392–5397.
  44. Herweg, N.A.; Solomon, E.A.; Kahana, M.J. Theta Oscillations in Human Memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020, 24, 208–227.
  45. Lega, B.C.; Jacobs, J.; Kahana, M. Human Hippocampal Theta Oscillations and the Formation of Episodic Memories. Hippocampus 2012, 22, 748–761.
  46. Lin, J.J.; Rugg, M.D.; Das, S.; Stein, J.; Rizzuto, D.S.; Kahana, M.J.; Lega, B.C. Theta Band Power Increases in the Posterior Hippocampus Predict Successful Episodic Memory Encoding in Humans. Hippocampus 2017, 27, 1040–1053.
  47. Miller, J.; Watrous, A.J.; Tsitsiklis, M.; Lee, S.A.; Sheth, S.A.; Schevon, C.A.; Smith, E.H.; Sperling, M.R.; Sharan, A.; Asadi-Pooya, A.A.; et al. Lateralized Hippocampal Oscillations Underlie Distinct Aspects of Human Spatial Memory and Navigation. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 2423.
  48. Sederberg, P.B.; Kahana, M.J.; Howard, M.W.; Donner, E.J.; Madsen, J.R. Theta and Gamma Oscillations during Encoding Predict Subsequent Recall. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 10809–10814.
  49. Solomon, E.A.; Lega, B.C.; Sperling, M.R.; Kahana, M.J. Hippocampal Theta Codes for Distances in Semantic and Temporal Spaces. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 24343–24352.
  50. Fell, J.; Klaver, P.; Elfadil, H.; Schaller, C.; Elger, C.E.; Fernández, G. Rhinal–Hippocampal Theta Coherence during Declarative Memory Formation: Interaction with Gamma Synchronization? Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003, 17, 1082–1088.
  51. Gruber, M.J.; Hsieh, L.T.; Staresina, B.P.; Elger, C.E.; Fell, J.; Axmacher, N.; Ranganath, C. Theta Phase Synchronization between the Human Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex Increases during Encoding of Unexpected Information: A Case Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 30, 1646–1656.
  52. Ezzyat, Y.; Kragel, J.E.; Burke, J.F.; Levy, D.F.; Lyalenko, A.; Wanda, P.; O’Sullivan, L.; Hurley, K.B.; Busygin, S.; Pedisich, I.; et al. Direct Brain Stimulation Modulates Encoding States and Memory Performance in Humans. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, 1251–1258.
  53. Ezzyat, Y.; Wanda, P.A.; Levy, D.F.; Kadel, A.; Aka, A.; Pedisich, I.; Sperling, M.R.; Sharan, A.D.; Lega, B.C.; Burks, A.; et al. Closed-Loop Stimulation of Temporal Cortex Rescues Functional Networks and Improves Memory. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 365.
  54. Fellner, M.C.; Gollwitzer, S.; Rampp, S.; Kreiselmeyr, G.; Bush, D.; Diehl, B.; Axmacher, N.; Hamer, H.; Hanslmayr, S. Spectral Fingerprints or Spectral Tilt? Evidence for Distinct Oscillatory Signatures of Memory Formation. PLOS Biol. 2019, 17, e3000403.
  55. Greenberg, J.A.; Burke, J.F.; Haque, R.; Kahana, M.J.; Zaghloul, K.A. Decreases in Theta and Increases in High Frequency Activity Underlie Associative Memory Encoding. NeuroImage 2015, 114, 257–263.
  56. Kragel, J.E.; Ezzyat, Y.; Sperling, M.R.; Gorniak, R.; Worrell, G.A.; Berry, B.M.; Inman, C.; Lin, J.J.; Davis, K.A.; Das, S.R.; et al. Similar Patterns of Neural Activity Predict Memory Function during Encoding and Retrieval. NeuroImage 2017, 155, 60–71.
  57. Long, N.M.; Kahana, M.J. Successful Memory Formation Is Driven by Contextual Encoding in the Core Memory Network. NeuroImage 2015, 119, 332–337.
  58. Solomon, E.A.; Stein, J.M.; Das, S.; Gorniak, R.; Sperling, M.R.; Worrell, G.; Inman, C.S.; Tan, R.J.; Jobst, B.C.; Rizzuto, D.S.; et al. Dynamic Theta Networks in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe Support Episodic Memory. Curr. Biol. 2019, 29, 1100–1111.e4.
  59. Nan, W.; Rodrigues, J.P.; Ma, J.; Qu, X.; Wan, F.; Mak, P.I.; Mak, P.U.; Vai, M.I.; Rosa, A. Individual Alpha Neurofeedback Training Effect on Short Term Memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2012, 86, 83–87.
  60. Hosseini, S.M.H.; Pritchard-Berman, M.; Sosa, N.; Ceja, A.; Kesler, S.R. Task-Based Neurofeedback Training: A Novel Approach Toward Training Executive Functions. NeuroImage 2016, 134, 159.
  61. Liu, N.; Cliffer, S.; Pradhan, A.H.; Lightbody, A.; Hall, S.S.; Reiss, A.L. Optical-Imaging-Based Neurofeedback to Enhance Therapeutic Intervention in Adolescents with Autism: Methodology and Initial Data. Neurophotonics 2016, 4, 011003.
More
This entry is offline, you can click here to edit this entry!
Video Production Service