Aggression in Dogs: Comparison
Please note this is a comparison between Version 2 by Vivi Li and Version 1 by Paulina Cholewińska.

Aggression as a behavior is not always desirable, often ends in abandonment and/or euthanasia. However, it is possible to prevent the occurrence of unwanted aggression in domestic dogs. Aggression is not a fully understood phenomenon. To aim to generalize the dogs’ behavior and understand their behavioral needs, ethograms have been developed. Communication of any kind, including aggression, is a natural part of a dog’s ethogram. An ethogram is a species-specific list of natural behavior. Dog ethograms include affiliate, agonistic, defensive, sedative, sexual, demonstrative, warning, stressful, playful, grooming, exploratory, related to hunting, or related to the reduction of stress.

  • dogs
  • aggression
  • diet
  • hormones
  • neurocognitive science

1. Introduction

It is estimated that dogs are the first animal species domesticated by humans. As a result of selective breeding, about 400 different breeds of dogs are recognized around the world, representing a large variety in terms of size, use, or weight [1,2,3][1][2][3]. In addition, keeping dogs was associated with the need to learn about their behavior in order to maintain proper relations between the dog and the owner, as well as to eliminate the undesirable behavior, such as aggression [1,2,3,4][1][2][3][4].
To aim to generalize the dogs’ behavior and understand their behavioral needs, ethograms have been developed. Communication of any kind, including aggression, is a natural part of a dog’s ethogram. An ethogram is a species-specific list of natural behavior. Dog ethograms include affiliate, agonistic, defensive, sedative, sexual, demonstrative, warning, stressful, playful, grooming, exploratory, related to hunting, or related to the reduction of stress [5,6][5][6]. Additionally, in recent years, new methods have been developed to help understand how a dog’s brain works: neurocognition. The combination of the dietary aspect, physiology (endocrine system), and neurocognition may allow for a thorough understanding of the impact of aggression on the dog’s body and to obtain more detailed information on the basis of its occurrence [7,8,9,10][7][8][9][10].
Understanding the causes of the occurrence of aggression in dogs is crucial to both animal welfare and the safety of their caregivers. In the circumstances in which a dog shows undesirable aggression, they can inform the competent caregivers about the disposition of the animal, as well as take appropriate measures to eliminate aggressive behavior [4]. Additionally, in North America, aggressive behavior is strongly contributed toward the abandonment and/or euthanasia of animals, especially dogs [8]. Many types of aggression can be distinguished in dog behavior depending on motivation (defensive/offensive), target (other animals, owner, unfamiliar people), etc. In addition, the definition of the underlying cause of aggression has not been fully explained, as it can be both behavioral (related to unmet needs, especially in the early time of life, anxiety or stress, poor socialization, etc.), diseases (such as cancer, brain disorders or diet, etc.) or genetics [9,10,11,12,13,14,15][9][10][11][12][13][14][15]. Recently, apart from the use of appropriate training for behavioral disorders, the diet and physiology (including the endocrine system) of dogs have also come to be seen as an aspect of the disorders [14]. In particular, the focus was placed on nutritional additives such as fatty acids, amino acids, and probiotics.

2. Aggression—Definitions, Types, Consequences, and Possible Causes

Dog aggression can be divided depending on the mental state of the dog and the circumstances in which the aggression appears. Acts of aggression can be categorized against dogs, people, other animals, and moving objects, such as bicycles and cars [10,13,15][10][13][15] (Table 1).
Table 1.
 Selected aggression behavior definitions.
DEFENSIVE AGGRESSION
Occurs when the dog, due to the lack of socialization with other animals, reads other animals as deadly, painful procedures or their arrival are associated with pain and irritation, the safety limit for a bitch protecting her young is exceeded, as well as a safety limit for a dog protecting his area [16]
DISTANCING AGGRESSION
This is a symptom of social anxiety disorder. Through distancing aggression, the dog manifests pathological fear or anxiety in contact with other animals or dogs [17]
TERRITORIAL AGGRESSION
The goal of territorial aggression is to keep the threat at a safe distance. Defending a territory is a natural behavior for a dog, which is why dog owners praise those who warn against intruders and guard the property, which makes the problem of territorial aggression worsen in every situation of this type. Aggression towards postmen is the biggest factor that deepens this type of aggression due to the fact that the systematic appearance of the postman and the reward by the dog, which is the departure of the postman, strengthens this type of behaviors [9].
MATERNITY AGGRESSION
It is a type of defensive aggression of varying severity. Its aim is to chase away intruders who could threaten the puppies or injure the bitch, which may have a negative impact on the further rearing of the young. When the intruder is a stranger, the attack is brutal and direct. Maternal aggression resembles territorial and distancing aggression, because the female dog in most cases, fiercely defends not only the puppies but also the place where she gave birth or the place where the puppies usually live [18]
AGGRESSION FROM DISEASE
It is a type of defensive aggression of moderate intensity. This type of aggression is characteristic of dogs forced to do something, hurt or upset. A common stimulus is an illness or wound that requires care. Unfortunately, usually in such situations the dog’s guardian intervenes, who may become a victim of aggression, which may deteriorate the bond between him and the dog [19]
AGGRESSION OUT OF FEAR
It is a type of defensive aggression that is a natural behavior for any living being. This type of aggression caused by fear should be treated as an incident, not a habit, but it should be remembered that this type occurs during anxiety disorders, the basis of which should be found and eliminated [20]
DISPLACED AGGRESSION
It is aggression that can arise from any other type of aggression. A distinctive feature is a high agitation. In this case, the dog tries at all costs to transfer its agitation and aggression to the object closest to it. This type of aggression is especially unpredictable because a dog that is aggressive towards another animal may seek to vent its aggression by attacking its handler, who is within reach of his jaws. Displaced aggression is an automatic behavior that can be found in explosive and impulsive individuals [17]
COMPETITIVE AGGRESSION BETWEEN DOGS
This type of aggression is perceived as a game in which the participants, depending on their social positions, try to impress the opponent by adopting various poses. It should not end in serious injuries. The conflict allows you to verify which side is dominant and which side is defeated [15]
COMPETITIVE AGGRESSION BETWEEN DOGS AND PEOPLE
As in the case of competitive aggression between dogs, this time, it is a kind of game. Unfortunately, this time the man from above is in a losing position because he cannot take part in this game, which in turn may result in bodily harm. Not knowing how to read this type of aggression contributes to a wrong assessment of the situation by a human and rewarding the dog for dominating the owner, about which the dominated person himself has no idea [20]
POSSESIVE AGGRESSION
It occurs when the dog defends access to its own or stolen property, person, land, or food. It happens that by provoking the owner, the dog verifies its position in the hierarchy or tries to convince him to play [13]
AGGRESSION BETWEEN DOGS BELONGING TO DIFFERENT SOCIAL GROUPS
This type of aggression is used when dogs from different social groups meet outside their territories and have a strong need to establish a hierarchical position among themselves [15]
HUNTING AGGRESSION
Hunting aggression does not end with killing the victim. This type of aggression can be eliminated by properly socializing the dog with objects or situations such as runners or cyclists. As a predator, the dog reacts to moving objects or creatures by chasing, which is a natural behavior for him. As in the case of hunting aggression, there are breeds whose tendencies to chase are conditioned by genes and its utility [18]
HUNTER AGGRESSION
The case of this type of aggression is unique in that the behavior and the dog’s facial expressions are unlike any other type of aggression. The dog’s muzzle remains smooth, and the dog only becomes emotionally aroused in a manner characteristic only of hunting. As a predator, the dog can hunt anything that moves that has not been socialized with. The victim may be a creature or thing that the dog considers edible or not representative of its species. This also applies to small breeds of dogs when the aggressor is a representative of a large breed of dog. The occurrence of hunting aggression is largely determined by the genes and utility of the breed [18]
EXCESSIVE AGGRESSION
Excessive aggression is divided into secondary, primary, and command. In the case of excessive secondary aggression, it gradually transforms from other types of aggression, while in the case of primary excessive aggression, it appears suddenly. In both cases, wresearchers are dealing with pathology, and the tendency to these types of aggression may indicate an explosive personality or diseases such as brain tumors or schizophrenia. Behavior in both cases of excessive aggression is diagnosed as illogical and non-functional [21,22][21][22]
In this paper, however, the definition created by James O’Heare [20] will be used. The author summed up aggression as “an attack, a deliberate attack, or a threat of an attack on another individual”. The types of dog aggression can be categorized according to the sequence of behaviors and the circumstances in which they occur. The following types of aggression can be distinguished; defensive, distancing, territorial, maternal, irritable, fearful, displaced, competitive between dogs, competitive between dogs and people, possessive, possessive, between dogs belonging to different social groups, hunting, excessive, on command, etc. Figure 1 [17].
Figure 1. Selected types of aggression in dogs depend on motivation target [9,13,20,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32].
 Selected types of aggression in dogs depend on motivation target [9][13][20][23][24][25][26][27][28][29][30][31][32].
Aggression is a natural part of dog behavior, and it is part of the ethogram. However, excessive aggression disrupts the relationship between the dog and its environment [17,18,19,20][17][18][19][20]. Aggression can be based on motivational basis (territorial-, fear-, possessiveness-related, etc.) or target basis (stranger-, owner-, dog-directed, etc.) [33]. The change of motivation occurs most often when one of the dog’s needs has not been met. For example, in a hungry dog, the change of motivation will manifest itself in looking for food, devoting all the time to it, including the rest period. It will be accompanied by anxiety, distraction, and irritability. The change of motivation also occurs at the time of the onset of the disease. Pain can lead to an avoidance of touch and a closer relationship with the environment and prompts the animal to defend itself, thus showing aggressive behavior [10]. Frustration can change your motivation, which may occur when a reward for expected behavior is missing/delayed or when the dog cannot achieve the goal [34,35][34][35]. Frustration-induced behavior can vary depending on the situation and the nature of the dog. As a result, the dog may increase efforts to achieve the desired goal or vocalize [36,37][36][37]. Frustration has been found to be an important emotion that influences a number of behavioral problems in dogs. Barrier frustration may arise when the dog is unable to achieve the desired goal by restricting its accessibility, such as with a leash, which may result in diversion and/or aggressive behavior [37,38][37][38]. Studies carried out in the years 2008–2016 showed that aggression against unfamiliar dogs (aggression targeted at the dog) had been shown by 22–47% of studied individuals. In many cases of such aggression, the death of one of the dogs involved can be a consequence [39,40][39][40]. A study by Van der Borg et al. [41] on Rottweilers showed that as many as a quarter of dogs (mainly without pedigrees) showed behavior related to aggression targeted at both humans and animals. In addition, they were also, in most cases, aggressors in a given interaction. The occurrence of targeted aggression in dogs in such situations may be due to a low level of anxiety (when the aggressor is the dog). Some breeds originally used as protection and guard dogs have a natural predisposition to low levels of anxiety [42,43,44][42][43][44]. On the other hand, when a dog with a high level of fear is subjected to a “stressor” such as unfamiliar people, other pets (such as cats or dogs), or the environment, it may result in defensive aggression [43,44][43][44]. Aggression based on fear can also be related to defensive aggression. This aggression is related to the animal’s reaction to a factor that it considers to be a threat, causing fear. This type of factor can also cause physiological and behavioral changes. The main physiological changes are increases in blood pressure, changes in cortisol levels, body temperature, increases in heart rate and blood glucose levels. In terms of behavioral changes, activation takes place at this time of the sympathetic adrenal medullary axis and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal cortex axis [30,45,46,47][30][45][46][47]. The occurrence of aggression in dogs has consequences related to their abandonment or euthanasia. Additionally, in the case of some breeds, their keeping or breeding has been subject to legal regulations, such as American Pit Bull Terrier or Tosa Inu in most European countries [26,48,49,50][26][48][49][50]. The created lists of aggressive dog breeds are controversial and do not provide certainty about the possibility/risk of aggressive dog’s behavior. Therefore, the issues of risk assessment are important, primarily based on two main aspects: the first is the ability to predict dog bite risk factors in the general canine population, and the second is how to assess and predict future risk to society [51,52,53][51][52][53]. In a study by Notari et al. [45], it was suggested that aggressive behavior mainly related to early experience, bad socialization process, origins (genetics factor), and welfare of dogs can have play an important role in both intraspecific and interspecific aggression [20,44,54,55,56,57,58][20][44][54][55][56][57][58]. However, aggressive reactions may occur because of poor health and well-being. Dog aggression can be caused by behavioral and health problems. Failure to meet the basic needs related to the animal’s welfare (access to water, food, play, the possibility of socialization) may cause due to stress, aggressive behavior, as well as health problems [52,53][52][53]. In the absence of access to water or food, the dog may search for them in inappropriate places such as toilets or garbage cans. Strengthening inappropriate feeding behaviors (including the speed of food, amount of food given, bowl positioning, etc.) can lead to stomach twists, choking, aggression, fighting for food and defending the bowl, stress, and asking for food [22,57,58,59,60][22][57][58][59][60]. When it comes to meeting physiological needs, the dog should be provided with appropriate space. In the studies on dogs staying in the shelter, it was shown that increasing the space twice had a positive effect on the behavior of dogs. Similar results were also obtained by Clark et al. [61] and Hetts et al. [58], which in turn indicates that both genetic and environmental factors can influence a dog’s behavior [39,61,62][39][61][62]. Otherwise, stress may occur due to the need to urinate or defecate or due to anticipating the penalty that awaits the dog for urinating or defecating in the wrong place. By praising the dog for inappropriate behavior or the owner not reacting to such behavior, the dog repeats it. This behavior of the owner be the cause of the reinforcement of inappropriate behavior and lead to the formation of undesired habits and situations resulting from them, e.g., barking at the sound of the doorbell. Improper behavior may be strengthened accidentally and unknowingly, also during conscious, as the guardian might think, training with a clicker. If the dog’s praise occurs too long after the desired behavior, the owner may unwillingly reinforce the following one, thus strengthening unwanted behaviors [63]. Research conducted in recent years also suggests that significantly fewer behavioral problems were shown in dogs that were trained solely through rewards, compared to dogs that were trained using only some form of punishment or a combination of both. The potential negative behavioral effects of aversive training techniques have also been identified in other studies such as Schilder and van der Borg [60], Hiby et al. [22], or Blackwell et al. [39]. During the period of socialization, the dog should get to know and establish positive contact with as many things, situations, and living creatures as possible. Socialization should last a dog’s life. However, it is one of the most important stages in a puppy. Properly socialized puppies are less likely to exhibit undesirable behavior and can develop a positive, lifelong relationship with their owner. Socialization practices should be age-appropriate (start within a few days of birth and should continue into adulthood as previously mentioned). They should be designed to expose the dog in a controlled and enjoyable manner to the many types of experiences, people, and objects it may encounter during its lifetime. Individuals that are properly socialized as puppies are less likely to exhibit behavioral problems in adulthood, including aggression and fear. Otherwise, stress may be manifested in certain situations and coped with, for example, through aggression. Dogs that have undergone a socialization program are much more self-confident and can cope with stressful situations more easily than those that have been less socialized [64,65,66,67,68][64][65][66][67][68]. The psychosomatic background, on the other hand, is associated with somatic diseases that have an inhibitory and productive effect on behavior. The somatic diseases affect the previous factors, less intense or not manifested behaviors at all, for example, aggression, defecation, and urination in the wrong places, vocalization, food and water intake, or self-mutilation [10,69][10][69]. The occurrence of somatic diseases such as thyroid disease, cancer (especially those related to the nervous system) influences behavior by a general change of motivation. Diseases such as brain cancer can direct disturbance of the brain’s work, which may result in, among others, the occurrence of aggressive behavior. Diet can significantly influence the occurrence of aggressive behavior in dogs too [69,70,71,72,73][69][70][71][72][73].

3. Selected Aspects of Dogs’ Diet and the Occurrence of Aggression

A properly balanced is the basis for the proper functioning of the animal and its health. There are many specialist diets available on the market to support the treatment of, among others, renal failure, arthritis, or cognitive impairment syndrome. The amount of tryptophan in the diet has a great influence on the dog’s behavior, as this amino acid influences the activity of serotonin [14,73][14][73]. Studies in humans have long pointed to the influence of diet on behavior. A study conducted on children by Schoenthaler and Bier [69] found that a diet rich in vitamins and minerals can reduce antisocial behavior in school-aged children. Additionally, studies by Gesch et al. [73] confirmed these findings but also showed that supplementation with both vitamin and mineral supplements and essential fatty acids (SFA) such as docosahexaenoic acid—22: 6n-3 (DHA) reduced the occurrence of violence-related behaviors [73,74][73][74]. In the case of dogs, models of their behavior are also considered as a model for humans, and previous research indicates that diet plays an important role in the behavior of these animals [74]. For many years, the relationship between the amount of protein in the diet, including tryptophan metabolism, and aggressive behavior in dogs has been the subject of discussion and interest. Research by DeNapoli et al. [75] and Dodman et al. [76] showed that the level of protein in the dog’s diet is an important aspect in the treatment of canine behavioral disorders, particularly aggression. These studies were based on the use of two types of diets: high protein (HP) and low protein (LP) in dog nutrition. The use of an LP diet in combination with the addition of L-tryptophan (Trp) may be effective in reducing dominant aggression in dogs. The effect obtained with a low-protein diet may be related to obtaining an appropriate ratio of Trp to other large neutral amino acids (LNAA) in the blood plasma. In addition, a low-protein diet rich in carbohydrates may adversely affect the ratio of Trp and LNAA, similar to an HP diet. The imbalance between the above-mentioned amino acids may result in their increased competition for blood-brain transfer. Disruption of Trp transfer from the blood-brain pathway may reduce the production of serotonin (5-HT), which in turn may lead to impulsive aggression. L-tryptophan is a biosynthetic precursor of the neurotransmitter serotonin, as well as melatonin and niacin (B3). Therefore, it plays a key role in the diet of dogs and prevents the occurrence of, inter alia, impulsive aggression [77,78,79,80,81,82][77][78][79][80][81][82]. In addition, long-term supplementation at a dose of 1 g/dog per day resulted in an increase in voluntary food consumption, which also indicates its role in influencing the hormones involved in food intake and satiety regulation, such as leptin or cholecystokinin [82]. Another important aspect of the diet is the level of lipids, in particular the number of fatty acids such as eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), conjugated linoleic acid (CLA), or docosahexaenoic acid (DHA). As mentioned earlier, human studies have found a link between dietary DHA levels and aggressive behavior. Lipids perform many key functions, including they are components of cell membranes, precursors of chemical messengers, and are used as a source of energy or stored in adipose tissue. Additionally, they are highly concentrated in the central nervous system. Additionally, a component of the brain’s gray matter is derived from dietary CLA (18: 2n-6) and α-linolenic acid (18: 3n-3). As a result of an enzymatic reaction, linolenic acid can be converted in the body into arachidonic acid and then into docosapentaenoic acid (22: 5n-6). In contrast, α-linolenic acid gives EPA (20: 5n-3), which can then be metabolized to DHA (22: 6n-3) [83,84,85][83][84][85]. Brain membrane phospholipids contain high levels of polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA), especially arachidonic and docosahexanoic acids [85]. Dietary PUFA levels affect cognitive functions in mammals, including dogs [86,87][86][87]. Combinations of DHA and EPA in a study by Kidd et al. [86] showed that they have a significant impact on various psychological functions, such as protection against hyperactivity and aggression. In addition, many studies in mammals (mainly mice) suggest that an altered lipid profile may be responsible for the occurrence of aggression and impulsiveness in animals [87]. A study by Re et al. [84] on German Shepherds showed that low cholesterol, bilirubin, docosahexaenoic acid, and a higher omega-6/omega-3 ratio might be correlated with the occurrence of aggressiveness in the tested dogs. Confirmation of the obtained results by Re et al. [84] can also be found in Sentürk and Yalçin [85], where the lipid levels of aggressive dogs were found to be lower than those of non-aggressive dogs. Both studies suggest that this was associated with lowering cholesterol and triglycerides and HDL–C, which negatively affects serotonin reuptake but requires further investigation. Another important aspect of nutrition is the ability to influence the microbiome of the digestive system. Recent studies link the diversity of the composition of the gut microbiome with behavioral and psychological regulation in other mammals such as mice and humans, including aggressive behavior [88,89,90,91][88][89][90][91]. A study by Kirchoff et al. [91] on dogs of the pit bull type showed a correlation between the composition of the digestive system microbiome and the incidence of aggression in dogs. The authors of this study suggest the possibility of using specific probiotics in the future to stabilize the microbiome of aggressive dogs because, according to preliminary analyzes, the microbiome of aggressive dogs are characterized by a higher level of Firmicutes, Fusobacteria, Bacteroidetes, Proteobacteria phyla, including Lactobacillus, and a lower level of Bacteroides, however, it requires for further research [92,93][92][93]. In addition to probiotics, it is possible to administer substances of plant origin that may positively affect the microbiome of the digestive system, such as proanthocyanins derived from grapes. In a study by Scarsella et al. [93] where the aforementioned substance was used, it was shown that it affects the microflora of the digestive system in dogs, as well as the neuroendocrine response—an increase in serotonin levels after 28 days of using the supplement. The use of polyphenols in dog nutrition as a way of manipulation of the microbiome is associated with the supposition that intestinal microorganisms may be involved in the absorption of the polyphenolic structure due to the transformation into low molecular weight compounds [94,95][94][95]. However, this topic requires further research in dogs. However, it should be considered that most of the presented studies were carried out on a small number of individuals, which requires further research.

References

  1. Lorenz, K. So Kam der Mensch Auf Den Hund; Deutscher Taschenbuch Verlag GmbH & Co., KG: München, Germany, 1983; pp. 1–242.
  2. Vilà, C.; Savoleinen, P.; Maldonado, J.E.; Amorim, I.R.; Rice, J.E.; Honeycutt, R.L.; Crandall, K.A.; Lundeberg, J.; Wayne, R.K. Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Science 1997, 276, 1687–1689.
  3. Savolainen, P.; Zhang, Y.P.; Luo, J.; Lundeberg, J.; Leitner, T. Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin of domestic dogs. Science 2002, 298, 1610–1613.
  4. Caffrey, N.; Rock, M.; Schmidtz, O.; Anderson, D.; Parkinson, M.; Checkley, S.L. Insights about the Epidemiology of Dog Bites in a Canadian City Using a Dog Aggression Scale and Administrative Data. Animals 2019, 9, 324.
  5. Rugaas, T. On Talking Terms with Dogs: Calming Signals; Dogwise Publishing: Wenatchee, WA, USA, 2006.
  6. Siniscalchi, M.; D’Ingeo, S.; Minunno, M.; Quaranta, A. Communication in Dogs. Animals 2018, 8, 131.
  7. Feuerstein, N.L.; Terkel, J. Interrelationships of dogs (Canis familiaris) and cats (Felis catus L.) living under the same roof. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 113, 150–165.
  8. van Haaften, K.A.; Grigg, E.K.; Kolus, C.; Hart, L.; Kogan, L.R. A survey of dog owners’ perceptions on the use of psychoactive medications and alternatives for the treatment of canine behavior problems. J. Vet. Behav. 2020, 35, 27–33.
  9. Blackshaw, J.K. An overview of types of aggressive behaviour in dogs and methods of treatment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1991, 30, 351–361.
  10. Horwitz, D.F.; Mills, D.S. Manual of Canine and Feline Behavioural Medicine, 2nd ed.; BSAVA: Gloucester, UK, 2009; pp. 1–324.
  11. Wallis, L.J.; Iotchev, I.B.; Kubinyi, E. Assertive, trainable and older dogs are perceived as more dominant in multi-dog households. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0227253.
  12. Flint, H.E.; Coe, J.B.; Serpell, J.A.; Pearl, D.L.; Niel, L. Risk factors associated with stranger-directed aggression in domestic dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 197, 45–54.
  13. Jacobs, J.A.; Coe, J.B.; Pearl, D.L.; Widowski, T.M.; Niel, L. Factors associated with canine resource guarding behaviour in the presence of dogs: A cross-sectional survey of dog owners. Prev. Vet. Med. 2018, 161, 134–142.
  14. Packer, R.M.; Law, T.H.; Davies, E.; Zanghi, B.; Pan, Y.; Volk, H.A. Effects of a ketogenic diet on ADHD-like behavior in dogs with idiopathic epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2016, 55, 62–68.
  15. Lockwood, R. Ethology, ecology and epidemiology of canine aggression. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016; pp. 160–181.
  16. Mertens, P.A. Canine aggression. In Manual of Canine and Feline Behavioural Medicin; Horwitz, D.F., Mills, D.S., Eds.; BSAVA: Gloucester, UK, 2002; pp. 195–215.
  17. Dehasse, J. Le Chien Agressif; Publibook: Paris, France, 2003.
  18. Lezama-García, K.; Mariti, C.; Mota-Rojas, D.; Martínez-Burnes, J.; Barrios-García, H.; Gazzano, A. Maternal behaviour in domestic dogs. Int. J. Vet. Sci. Med. 2019, 7, 20–30.
  19. Kujala, M.V.; Kujala, J.; Carlson, S.; Hari, R. Dog experts’ bains distinguish socially relevant body postures similarly in dogs and humans. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e39145.
  20. O’Heare, J. Aggressive Behavior in Dogs: A comprehensive Technical Manual for Professionals, 3rd ed.; BehaveTech Publishing: Ottawa, ON, Canada, 2017; pp. 1–248.
  21. Patronek, G.J.; Sacks, J.J.; Delise, K.M.; Cleary, D.V.; Marder, A.R. Co-occurrence of potentially preventable factors in 256 dog bite-related fatalities in the United States (2000–2009). J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2013, 243, 1726–1736.
  22. Hiby, E.F.; Rooney, N.J.; Bradshaw, J.W.S. Dog training methods: Their use; effectiveness and interaction with behaviour and welfare. Anim. Welf. 2004, 12, 63–69.
  23. Pąsiek, M.; Majecka, K.; Pietraszewski, D. “Pies Baskervillów”-czyli o naturze zachowań agresywnych u psa domowego canis lupus familiaris. Kosmos, Probl. Nauk. Biol. 2015, 64, 239–246.
  24. Gobbo, E.; Zupan, M. Dogs’ sociability, owners’ neuroticism and attachment style to pets as predictors of dog aggression. Animals 2020, 10, 315.
  25. Wright, J.C.; Nesselrote, M.S. Classification of behavior problems in dogs: Distributions of age, breed, sex and reproductive status. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1987, 19, 169–178.
  26. Stellato, A.C.; Flint, H.E.; Dewey, C.E.; Widowski, T.M.; Niel, L. Risk-factors associated with veterinary-related fear and aggression in owned domestic dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 241, 105374.
  27. Hsu, Y.; Sun, L. Factors associated with aggressive responses in pet dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 123, 108–123.
  28. Schilder, M.B.; van der Borg, J.A.; Vinke, C.M. Intraspecific killing in dogs: Predation behavior or aggression? A study of aggressors, victims, possible causes, and motivations. J. Vet. Behav. 2019, 34, 52–55.
  29. Available online: https://www.nidirect.gov.uk/articles/banned-breeds-dogs-northern-ireland (accessed on 24 August 2021).
  30. Mikkola, S.; Salonen, M.; Puurunen, J.; Hakanen, E.; Sulkama, S.; Araujo, C.; Lohi, H. Aggressive behaviour is affected by demographic, environmental and behavioural factors in purebred dogs. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1–10.
  31. Kuhne, F. Behavioural responses of dogs to dog-human social conflict situations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 182, 38–43.
  32. Available online: https://www.zoll.de/EN/Private-individuals/Travel/Entering-Germany/Restrictions/Dangerous-dogs/dangerous-dogs.html (accessed on 24 August 2021).
  33. Houpt, K.A. Terminology Think Tank: Terminology of aggressive behavior. J. Vet. Behav. 2006, 1, 39–41.
  34. Amsel, A. Frustration Theory: An Analysis of Dispositional Learning and Memory; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1992.
  35. McPeake, K.J.; Collins, L.M.; Zulch, H.; Mills, D.S. Behavioural and Physiological Correlates of the Canine Frustration Questionnaire. Animals 2021, 11, 3346.
  36. Jakovcevic, A.; Elgier, A.M.; Mustaca, A.E.; Bentosela, M. Frustration behaviors in domestic dogs. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2013, 16, 19–34.
  37. Mills, D.S.; Dube, M.B.; Zulch, H. Stress and Pheromonatherapy in Small Animal Clinical Behaviour; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012.
  38. Bowen, J.; Heath, S. Behavior Problems in Small Animals: Practical Advice for the Veterinary Team; Elsevier Health Sciences: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2005.
  39. Blackwell, E.J.; Twells, C.; Seawright, A.; Casey, R.A. The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2008, 3, 207–217.
  40. Casey, R.A.; Bradshaw, J.W.S. A Comparison of Referred Feline Clinical Behavior Cases with General Population Prevalence Data; Scientific Proceedings of the British Small Animal Veterinary Association Congress: Birmingham, UK, 2001; p. 529.
  41. Van der Borg, J.A.; Graat, E.A.; Beerda, B. Behavioural testing based breeding policy reduces the prevalence of fear and aggression related behaviour in Rottweilers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 195, 80–86.
  42. Camps, T.; Amat, M.; Manteca, X. A Review of Medical Conditions and Behavioral Problems in Dogs and Cats. Animals 2019, 9, 1133.
  43. Demirbas, Y.S.; Safak, E.; Emre, B.; Piskin, İ.; Ozturk, H.; Pereira, G.D.G. Rehabilitation program for urban free–ranging dogs in a shelter environment can improve behavior and welfare. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 18, 1–6.
  44. Karatsoreos, I.N.; McEwen, B.S. Psychobiological Allostasis: Resistance, Resilience and Vulnerability. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 576–584.
  45. Notari, L.; Cannas, S.; Di Sotto, Y.A.; Palestrini, C. A Retrospective Analysis of Dog–Dog and Dog–Human Cases of Aggression in Northern Italy. Animals 2020, 10, 1662.
  46. Clay, L.; Paterson, M.B.A.; Bennett, P.; Perry, G.; Phillips, C.C.J. Do Behaviour Assessments in a Shelter Predict the Behaviour of Dogs Post-Adoption? Animals 2020, 10, 1225.
  47. Reisner, I.R.; Shofer, F.S.; Nance, M.L. Behavioral assessment of child-directed canine aggression. Inj. Prev. 2007, 13, 348–351.
  48. Laws about Potentially Dangerous Dogs. Available online: https://andaluciarealty.com/blog/pets-in-spain-laws/ (accessed on 24 August 2021).
  49. Davidson, D. Dangerous Dogs. Act. Veter. Rec. 1995, 136, 275.
  50. Westgarth, C.; Watkins, F. A qualitative investigation of the perceptions of female dog-bite victims and implications for the prevention of dog bites. J. Veter. Behav. 2015, 10, 479–488.
  51. Redlicka, A. Rottweiler: Kupno, Żywienie, Pielęgnacja, Charakter, Zdrowie, Rozmnażanie i Wiele Więcej; Galaktyka: Łódź, Poland, 2005; pp. 1–64.
  52. Moberg, G.P. Biological Response to Stress: Implications for Animal Welfare. In The Biology of Animal Stress: Basic Principles and Implications for Animal Welfare; Mobert, G.P., Mench, I.A., Eds.; CABI: Wallingford, UK, 2000; pp. 1–22.
  53. Netto, W.J.; Planta, D.J. Behavioural testing for aggression in the domestic dog. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1997, 52, 243–263.
  54. Horn, L.; Range, F.; Huber, L. Dog’s attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity. Anim. Cogn. 2013, 16, 435–443.
  55. Rooney, N.; Gaines, S.; Hiby, E. A practitioner’s guide to working dog welfare. J. Veter. Behav. 2009, 4, 127–134.
  56. Clark, J.N.; Daurio, C.P.; Barth, D.W.; Batty, A.F. Evaluation of a beef-based chewable formulation of pyrantel pamoate against induced and natural infections of hookworms and ascarids in dogs. Vet. Parasitol. 1991, 40, 127–133.
  57. Hetts, S.; Clark, J.D.; Calpin, J.P.; Arnold, C.E.; Mateo, J.M. Influence of housing conditions on beagle behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1992, 34, 137–155.
  58. Buffington, C.A.T.; Westropp, J.L.; Chew, D.J.; Bolus, R.R. Clinical evaluation of multimodal environmental modification (MEMO) in the management of cats with idiopathic cystitis.J. Feline Med. Surg. J. Feline Med. Surg. 2006, 8, 261–268.
  59. Schilder, M.B.; van der Borg, J. A Training dogs with help of the shock collar: Short and long term behavioural effects. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004, 85, 319–334.
  60. Cline, K.M.C. Psychological effects of dog ownership: Role strain, role enhancement, and depression. J. Soc. Physol. 2010, 150, 117–131.
  61. Glenk, L.M. Current perspectives on therapy dog welfare in animal-assisted interventions. Animals 2007, 7, 7.
  62. Case, L.P. The Dog: Its Behavior, Nutrition, and Health, 2nd ed.; Wiley-Blackwell Publishing: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 1–496.
  63. Howell, T.J.; King, T.; Bennett, P.C. Puppy parties and beyond: The role of early age socialization practices on adult dog behavior. Vet. Med. Res. Rep. 2015, 6, 143.
  64. Batt, L.; Batt, M.; Baguley, J.; McGreevy, P. The effects of structured sessions for juvenile training and socialization on guide dog success and puppy-raiser participation. J. Veter. Behav. 2008, 3, 199–206.
  65. Jones, P.; Chase, K.; Davern, A.P.; Ostrander, E.A.; Lark, K.G. Single-nucleotide-polymorphism-based association mapping of dog stereotypes. Genetics 2008, 179, 1033–1044.
  66. Schroll, S.; Dehasse, J. Haltensmedizin beim Hund. Leitsymptome, Diagnostik, Therapie und Prävention; Enke Verlag GmbH: Stuttgart, Germany, 2016; pp. 1–322.
  67. McFarland, D. Animal Behaviour Psychobiology, Ethology and Evolution, 3rd ed.; Prentice Hall: Harlow, UK; Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1999; pp. 1–580.
  68. Schoenthaler, S.J.; Bier, I.D. The effect of vitamin-mineral supplementation on juvenile delinquency among American schoolchildren: A randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2000, 6, 7–17.
  69. Parsons, E. Click to Calm; Sunshine Books: Waltham, MA, USA, 2004; pp. 1–181.
  70. Aloff, B. Aggression in Dogs: Practical Management, Prevention and Behaviour Modification; Funderaft Publishing: Collierville, TN, USA, 2002; pp. 1–418.
  71. Overall, K. Clinical Behavioral Medicine for Small Animals; Mosby: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 1–560.
  72. Gesch, C.B.; Hammond, S.M.; Hampson, S.E.; Eves, A.; Crowder, M.J. Influence of supplementary vitamins, minerals and essential fatty acids on the antisocial behaviour of young adult prisoners. Randomised, placebo-controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 2002, 181, 22–28.
  73. Hibbeln, J.R.; Umhau, J.C.; Linnoila, M.; George, D.T.; Ragan, P.W.; Shoaf, S.E.; Vaughan, M.R.; Rawlings, R.; Salem, N., Jr. A replication study of violent and nonviolent subjects: Cerebrospinal fluid metabolites of serotonin and dopamine are predicted by plasma essential fatty acids. Biol. Psychiatry 1998, 44, 243–249.
  74. DeNapoli, J.S.; Dodman, N.H.; Shuster, L.; Rand, W.M.; Gross, K.L. Effect of dietary protein content and tryptophan supplementation on dominance aggression, territorial aggression, and hyperactivity in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Asoc. 2000, 217, 504–508.
  75. Dodman, N.H.; Reisner, I.; Shuster, L.; Rand, W.; Luescher, U.A.; Robinson, I.; Houpt, K.A. Effect of dietary protein content on behavior in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 1996, 208, 376–379.
  76. Fragua, V.; González-Ortiz, G.; Villaverde, C.; Hervera, M.; Maria Mariotti, V.; Manteca, X.; Baucells, D.M. Preliminary study: Voluntary food intake in dogs during tryptophan supplementation. Br. J. Nutr. 2011, 106 (Suppl. 1), S162–S165.
  77. Riggio, G.; Mariti, C.; Sergi, V.; Diverio, S.; Gazzano, A. Serotonin and Tryptophan Serum Concentrations in Shelter Dogs Showing Different Behavioural Responses to a Potentially Stressful Procedure. Vet. Sci. 2021, 8, 1.
  78. Carrié, I.; Clement, M.; de Javel, D.; Francès, H.; Bourre, J.M. Specific phospholipid fatty acid composition of brain regions in mice. Effects of n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid deficiency and phospholipid supplementation. J. Lipid Res. 2000, 41, 465–472.
  79. Lauritzen, L.; Hansen, H.S.; Jørgensen, M.H.; Michaelsen, K.F. The essentiality of long chain n-3 fatty acids in relation to development and function of the brain and retina. Prog. Lipid Res. 2001, 40, 1–94.
  80. Wainwright, P.E. Do essential fatty acids play a role in brain and behavioral development? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1992, 16, 193–205.
  81. Gajos, J.M.; Beaver, K.M. The effect of omega-3 fatty acids on aggression: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Res. 2016, 69, 147–158.
  82. Kaplan, J.R.; Shively, C.A.; Fontenot, M.B.; Morgan, T.M.; Howell, S.M.; Manuck, S.B.; Muldoon, M.F.; Mann, J.J. Demonstration of an association among dietary cholesterol, central serotonergicactivity, and social behavior in monkeys. Psychom. Med. 1994, 56, 479–484.
  83. Re, S.; Zanoletti, M.; Emanuele, E. Aggressive dogs are characterized by low omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid status. Vet. Res. Comm. 2008, 32, 225–230.
  84. Sentürk, S.; Yalçin, E. Hypocholesterolaemia in dogs with dominance aggression. J. Vet. Med. A Physiol. Pathol. Clin. Med. 2003, 50, 339–342.
  85. Kidd, P.M. Omega-3 DHA and EPA for cognition, behavior, and mood: Clinical findings and structural-functional synergies with cell membrane phospholipids. Altern. Med. Rev. 2007, 12, 207.
  86. Cryan, J.F.; O’Mahony, S.M. The microbiome-gut-brain axis: From bowel to behavior. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 2011, 23, 187–192.
  87. Mayer, M.P.; Odenthal, B.; Faber, M.; Winkelholz, C.; Schlick, C.M. Cognitive engineering of automated assembly processes. Hum. Factors Ergon. Manuf. Serv. Ind. 2014, 24, 348–368.
  88. Foster, J.A.; Lyte, M.; Meyer, E.; Cryan, J.F. Gut microbiota and brain function: An evolving field in neuroscience. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016, 19, pyv114.
  89. Sylvia, K.E.; Jewell, C.P.; Rendon, N.M.; St John, E.A.; Demas, G.E. Sex-specific modulation of the gut microbiome and behavior in Siberian hamsters. Brain Behav. Immun. 2017, 60, 51–62.
  90. Kirchoff, N.S.; Udell, M.A.; Sharpton, T.J. The gut microbiome correlates with conspecific aggression in a small population of rescued dogs (Canis familiaris). PeerJ 2019, 7, e6103.
  91. Mondo, E.; Barone, M.; Soverini, M.; D’Amico, F.; Cocchi, M.; Petrulli, C.; Mattioli, M.; Marliani, G.; Candela, M.; Accorsi, P.A. Gut microbiome structure and adrenocortical activity in dogs with aggressive and phobic behavioral disorders. Heliyon 2020, 6, e03311.
  92. Scarsella, E.; Cintio, M.; Iacumin, L.; Ginaldi, F.; Stefanon, B. Interplay between Neuroendocrine Biomarkers and Gut Microbiota in Dogs Supplemented with Grape Proanthocyanidins: Results of Dietary Intervention Study. Animals 2020, 10, 531.
  93. Russell, W.R.; Scobbie, L.; Labat, A.; Duthie, G.G. Selective bio-availability of phenolic acids from Scottish strawberries. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2009, 53, S85–S91.
  94. Warnes, C. Five myths commonly associated with neutering in dogs. Vet. Nurs. 2014, 5, 502–508.
  95. León, M.; Rosado, B.; García-Belenguer, S.; Chacón, G.; Villegas, A.; Palacio, J. Assessment of serotonin in serum, plasma, and platelets of aggressive dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2012, 7, 348–352.
More
Video Production Service