Compensatory Carry-Over Action Model (CCAM): Comparison
Please note this is a comparison between Version 4 by Sonia Lippke and Version 7 by Dean Liu.

The CompensatoryCompensatory Carry-Over Action Model Carry-Over Action Model (CCAM) is innovative as most behavioral theories only model single activity. The CCAM, however, models different single activities—such as physical activity and nNutrition —and how they change as a result of one another. Such lifestyle activities are assumed to be formed by higher-level goals, which can drive activity volitionally or unconsciously, and are rather unspecific. They become specific because of activities that are subjectively seen as leading to this goal.  Each activity must be intended, pursued, and controlled. Specific resources ensure that individuals have the chance to translate their intentions into activity, and that they resist distractors. Compensation and transfer (also called carry-over) operate between the different activities. If people devote all of their energy to one domain and believe that no resources remain for the other activity, compensation can help to attain goals. It is also possible that an individual successfully performs one activity, and existing or developing resources may be transferred to another activity.

  • Multiple Behavior Change
  • Physical Activity
  • Physical Excerise
  • Nutrition
  • Health Behavior
  • Work-Life Balance

1. Introduction

Most goals in life (e.g., becoming/remaining a high performing scientist or/and staying healthy, a so-called higher-level goal) can only be reached by means of more than one behavior (e.g., to work effectively and also to detach from work adequately by means of regular physical activity). At the same time, experiences and health outcomes like well-being result from such different behaviors. This is the main idea of the CCAM, which is displayed in Figure 1.

 

 

Figure CCAM Structure

Figure 1. The CCAM (Lippke, 2014, 2019).

 

As there are very few other theories explaining explicitly such complex behavior change. Thus, the CCAM Lippke (2014, 2019) is unique in terms of explaining and predicting multiple (health) behaviors jointly. The CCAM is based on other social-cognitive models which assume that behavior must be intended, planned, and translated into concrete activities. Higher-level goals can in turn determine what individuals experience in terms of outcome from the behaviors. Thereby they further control behavior adoption or maintenance via goal setting, planning but also self-efficacy beliefs.

Compensatory cognitions (CC) start operating in case of tempting situation (e.g., having to work over hours): If behavior A (e.g., exercising appropriately after work) is hindered by behavior B (e.g., work hard), one can decide to perform another behavior instead of behavior A (behavior C; e.g., do active commuting). Alternatively, one can adapt performance of behavior A by either executing it later or in a different way (e.g., exercising the next morning or just later and with a shorter duration). Based on this idea, individuals who perform a risk behavior may believe that they can compensate for this by performing another behavior or the planned behavior at a later point in time. However, many individuals experience problems with the intended compensatory behavior and end up with not performing the behavior A at all or behavior C. They may regret this, may not reach their higher level goal of becoming/remaining a high performing scientist, because they cannot work hard only but also need to detach from work by physical activity. Thus, they may also fall ill in the long run or may question whether striving for the goal of becoming/remaining a high performing scientist makes sense and give up on investing maximum energy into it. Altogether, the main experience may be that the well-being is lowered unless a new higher-level goal is set or the behaviors are optimized in concert. Thus, to help individuals to reach their goals requires the view on different behaviors.

Individuals with strong habits are less at risk of being distracted from their intended behavior. Thus, one can distinguish two groups: group 1 with those individuals who are experienced with a behavior, and group 2 with those for whom the behavior is completely new. Whereas group 1 has a higher likelihood of successfully translating intentions into behavior, group 2, without previous behavior experience, has to invest more volitional control, and is more at risk for not translating intentions based on compensatory cognitions (CC) into action.

In that sense, also reviewing ones’ compensatory intentions retrospectively may serve the purpose of understanding how compensating for the non-performance of the originally intended behavior actually lead to giving up the intention of the originally intended behavior. The key is that the awareness of ending up with a unhealthy lifestyle instead of the intended physically active lifestyle which also serves the purpose of being a high performing scientist by means of detaching from work, can help prioritizing higher-level goals and behaviors.

The intentions to perform the different behaviors must reach at least a moderate degree: The individual has to intend to perform the behavior sufficiently, performing the behavior even in face of temptations. Previous intentions and behavioral experiences (also called ‘stages of change’) come into play. Individuals with high intentions but no previous behavior performance and success experiences are those individuals most at risk for not translating their healthy intentions into behavior. Accordingly, helping individuals to have positive experiences is key. When the intended or needed behavior is not providing this for the individual (e.g., jogging due to knee problems with subsequent pain) then an alternative behavior enactment is needed to identify (e.g., nordic walking, swimming). There are many examples which can be found by experts. However, the key component is individualization and personalization: The personally or individually fitting behavior, situation and time, built and social environment has to be found. Action planning is the behavior change technique which aims for helping with that. However, which coping planning is well known and research, rather rarely the hindering function of different behaviors such as work and physical activity is considered. This needs to be done by means of a sophisticated perspective taking multiple behaviors into account.

 

Evidence for these cognitive processes demonstrates that compensatory cognitions are generally negative for adherence to the goal behavior (e.g., the exercise regime). However, intrinsic motivation can lower the risks for a lapse. And positive experiences from managing challenges such as a lapse, can be carried over from one behavior to another. Carry-over is also known as transfer effects. For instance, if an individual manages to work long hours but then also manages to perform physical exercise, this may increase to believe to be able to manage difficulties in general. Next time when a challenging work task arises, the individual approaches this task more efficiently due to a higher self-efficacy in general. Or the other way around, a generally hard working scientist may also have more confident to overcome temptations to exercise physically even when feeling tired or low. In other words, carry-over are mechanisms which help to carrying over resources from one domain to another or in terms of one behavior serving as a gateway for another. Generally, experience, skills, knowledge, and self-efficacy can be carried over from one behavior or its predictors to another.

Higher-level goals may volitionally or unconsciously regulate different behaviors and their predictors by readjusting the priorization. Overall, the CCAM consists of five assumptions, which are as follows.

(1) Different behaviors (such as work effectively and perform physical activity regularly) interrelate.

(2) Higher-level goals (e.g., becoming a high performing scientist or/and staying healthy) drive different behaviors by initiating and strengthening behavior-specific intentions (e.g., work hard as a scientist and staying healthy; perform physical activity on a regular basis).

(3) Within each behavior, one translates intentions into behavior via planning. Self-efficacy functions as a moderator of planning, and also directly supports behavior enactment.

(4) Behavior-specific processes for behavior A (physical activity) and behavior B (being a high performing scientist) interrelate via carry-over mechanisms and via compensation or compensatory cognitions.

(5) A healthy lifestyle consists of multiple behaviors, which buffer the stress response (e.g., due to chronic health limitations or disabilities, an acute infection or environmental challenges) and increase well-being.

Single studies support specific assumptions (see reference below). There is much evidence that different behaviors interrelate. Different studies support the assumption that carry-over mechanisms exist, and showing that cognitive carry-over and behavioral outcomes depend on, for instance, whether physical activity resources are being transferred to nutrition behavior. Research is still needed, however, to extend assumptions of the CCAM. This required by using different research designs, particularly longitudinal observations, Randomized Control Trials with experimentally testing effects, and complex analyses of different behaviors and how they change depending on each other.

 

References

Most goals Cin life (e.g., becoming/remaining a high performing scientist or/and staying healthy, a so-called higher-level goal) can only be reached by means of more than one behavior (e.g., to work effectively and also to detach from work adequately by means of regular physical activity). At the same time, experiences and health outcomes like well-being result from such different behaviors. This is the mainhlar, V., & Lippke, S. (2017). Physical Activity Behavior and Competing Activities: Interrelations in 55-to 70-Year-Old Germans. ideaJournal of aging and physical activity, of the CCAM25(4), which is displayed in Figure 1576-586.

Figure CCAM Structure

Figure 1. The CCAM (Lippke, 2014, 2019).

As thGerlle are very few other theories explaining explicitly such complex behavior change. Thus, the CCAM Lippke (2014, 2019) is unique in terms of explaining and predictingr, K., Lippke, S., & Nigg, C. R. (2017). Future directions of multiple (health) behaviors jointly. The CCAM is based on other social-cognitive models which assume that behavior change research. must be intendedJournal of behavioral medicine, planned40(1), and translated into concrete activities194-202. H

Ligher-level goals can in turn determine what individuals experience in terms of the outcome from the behaviors. Thereby they further control behavior adoption or maintenance via goal setting, planning but also self-efficacy beliefs.

Comng, W., Duan, Y. P., Shang, B. R., Wang, Y. P., Hu, C., & Lippkensatory cognitions (CC) start operating in case of a tempting situation (e.g., having to work over hours): If behavior A (e.g., exercising appropriately after work) is hindered by behavior B (e.g., work hard), one can decide to perform another behavior instead of behavior A (behavior C; e.g., do active commuting). Alternatively, one can adapt the performance of behavior A by either executing it later or in a different way (e.g., exercising the next morning or just later and with a shorter duration). Based on this idea, individuals who perform a risk behavior may believe that they can compensate for this by performing another behavior or the planned behavior at a later point in time, S. (2019). A web-based lifestyle intervention program for Chinese college students: study protocol and baseline characteristics of a randomized placebo-controlled trial. HoweverBMC public health, many19(1), 1097.

Lindividuals experience problems with the intended compensatory behavior and end up not performing behavior A at all or pke, S. (2014). Modelling and supporting complex behavior C. They may regret this, may not reach their higher -level goal of becoming/remaining a high performing scientist, because they cannot work hard only but also need to detach from work by physical activity. Thus, they may also fall ill in the long run or may question whether striving for the goal of becoming/remaining a high performing scientist makes sense and give up on investing maximum energy into it. Altogether, the main experience may be that the well-being is lowered unless a new higher-level goal is set or the behaviors arechange related to obesity and diabetes prevention and management with the Compensatory Carry-Over Action Model. oJournal of Diabetes & Obesity, 1(2), 1-5. https://www.ommegaonline.org/article-details/Modelling-and-Supporting-Complex-Behavior-Change-Related-to-Obesity-and-Diabetes-Prevention-and-Management-with-the-Compensatory-Carry-over-Action-Model/195

Lipptimizked i, S. (2019). Compensatory Carry-Over Action Model. In Haconcert. Thus, to help individuals to reach their goals requires a view of different behaviorkfort, D., Schinke, R. J., & Strauss, B. (Eds.

Individuals). wDith strong habits are less at risk of being distracted from their intended behavior. Thus, one can distinguish two groups: group 1 with those individuals who are experienced with a behaviorctionary of sport psychology: sport, exercise, and group 2 with those for whom the behavior is completely new. Whereas group 1 has a higher likelihood of successfully translating intentions into behavperforming arts (pp. 53). London, UK: Academic Press/Elsevier.

Lior; groupp 2, without previous behavior experience, has to invest more volitional control and is more at risk for not translating intentions based on compensatory cognitions (CC) into action.

In tke, S., & Cihlar, V. (2020). Social Participation during that sense, also reviewing ones’ compensatory intentions retrospectively may serve the purpose of understanding how compensating for the non-performance of the originally intended behavior actually lead to giving up the intention of the originally intended behavior. The key is that the awareness of ending up with an unhealthy lifestyle instead of the intended p Transition to Retirement: Findings on Work, Health and Physically active lifestyle which also serves the purpose of being a high -performing scientist by means of detaching Activity beyond Retirement from work can help to prioritize higher-level goals and behaviors.

Than Interview intentions to perform the different behaviors must reach at least a moderate degree: The individual has to intend to perform the behaviorStudy over the Course of 3 Years. sufficientlyActivities, Adaptation & Aging, 1-24.

Lippkerforming the behavior even in face of temptations. Previous intentions and behavioral experiences (also called 'stages of change') come into play. Individuals with high intentions but no previous behavior performance and success experiences are those individuals most at risk for not translating their healthy intentions into behavior. Accordingly, helping, S., & Schüz, B. (2019). Modelle gesundheitsbezogenen Handelns und Verhaltensänderung. In individualsGesundheitswissenschaften to have (positive experiences is key. When the intended or needed behavior is not providing this for the individual (e.g., jogging due to knee problems with subsequent pain) then an alternap. 299-310). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.

Stive behavior enactment is needed to identify (e.g., nordic walking, swimming). There are many examples that can be found by experts. However, the key component is individualization and personalization: The personally or individually fitting behavior, situation and time, built and social environment has to be found. Action planning is the behavior change technique that aims for helping with that. However, which coping planning is well known and research, rather rarely the hindering function of different behaviors such as work and physical activity is considered. This needs to be done by means of sophisticated perspective -taking multiple behaviors into account.

Evidence fm, V., Reinwand, D., Wienert, J., Kuhlmann, T., De Vries, H., & Lippke, S. (2017). Brief report: Compensatory these cognitive processes demonstrates that compensatory cognitionhealth beliefs are generally negative for adherence to the goal behavior (e.g., the exercise regime). However, intrinsic motivation can lower the risks of a lapse. And positive experiences from managing challenges such as a lapse can be carried over from one behavior to another. Carry-over is also known as transfer effects. For instance, if an individual manages to work long hours but then also manages to perform physical exercise, this may increase the belief to be able to manage difficulties in general. Next time when a challenging work task arises, the individual approaches this task more efficiently due to a higherely associated with intentions for regular fruit and vegetable consumption when self-efficacy in general. Or the other ways low. aroundJournal of health psychology, a22(8), gener1094-1100.

Tally hard -working scientist may also have more confidence to overcome temptations to exercise physically even when feeling tired or low. In other words, carry-over are mechanisms that help to carry over resources from one domain to another or in terms of one behavior serving as a gateway for another. Generally, experience, skills, knowledge, and self-efficacy can be carried over from one behavior or its predictors to another, S. L., Storm, V., Reinwand, D. A., Wienert, J., de Vries, H., & Lippke, S. (2018).

2. Five Assumptions

Higher-level goals may volitioUnally or unconsciously regulate different behaviors and their predictors by readjustiderstanding the prioritization. Overall, the CCAM consists of five assumptions, which are as follows.

(1) Diositive associations offerent behaviors (such as work effectively and performsleep, physical activity regularly) interrelate.

(2) Higher-level goals (e.g., becoming a high performing scientist or/and staying healthy) drive different behaviors by initiating and strengthening behavior-specific intentions (e.g., work hard as a scientist and staying healthy; perform physical activity on a regular basis).

(3) Within each behavior, one transruit and vegetable intake as predictors of qualates intentions into behavior via planning. Self-efficacy functions as a moderator of planning, and also directly supports behavior enactment.

(4)ty of life and subjective Behavior-specific processes for behavior A (physical activity) and behavior B (being a high performing scientist) interrelate via carry-over mechanisms and via compensation or compensatory cognitions.

(5) A healthy lifestyle consistsealth across age groups: a theory of multiple behaviors, which buffer the stress response (e.g., due to chronic health limitations or disabilities, an acute infection, or environmental challenges) and increase well-being.

Singleased, cross-sectional web-based studiesy. supportFrontiers in psychology, specific9, 977.

Tassumptions (see reference below). There is much evidence that different behaviors interrelate. Different studies support the assumption that carry-over mechanisms exist and showing that cognitive carry-over and behavioral outcomes depend on, for instance, whether physical activity resources are being transferred to nutrition behavior. Research is still needed, however, to extend assumptions of the CCAM, S. L., Whittal, A., & Lippke, S. (2018). Associations among Sleep, Diet, Quality of Life, and Subjective Health. This is required by using different research designsHealth Behavior and Policy Review, particularly longitudinal observations5(2), Randomized Control Trials with experimentally testing effects, and complex analyses of different behaviors and how they change depending on each other46-58.

References

  1. Cihlar, V., & Lippke, S. (2017). Physical Activity Behavior and Competing Activities: Interrelations in 55-to 70-Year-Old Germans. Journal of aging and physical activity, 25(4), 576-586.
  2. Geller, K., Lippke, S., & Nigg, C. R. (2017). Future directions of multiple behavior change research. Journal of behavioral medicine, 40(1), 194-202.
  3. Liang, W., Duan, Y. P., Shang, B. R., Wang, Y. P., Hu, C., & Lippke, S. (2019). A web-based lifestyle intervention program for Chinese college students: study protocol and baseline characteristics of a randomized placebo-controlled trial. BMC public health, 19(1), 1097.
  4. Lippke, S. (2014). Modelling and supporting complex behavior change related to obesity and diabetes prevention and management with the Compensatory Carry-Over Action Model. Journal of Diabetes & Obesity, 1(2), 1-5. https://www.ommegaonline.org/article-details/Modelling-and-Supporting-Complex-Behavior-Change-Related-to-Obesity-and-Diabetes-Prevention-and-Management-with-the-Compensatory-Carry-over-Action-Model/195
  5. Lippke, S. (2019). Compensatory Carry-Over Action Model. In Hackfort, D., Schinke, R. J., & Strauss, B. (Eds.). Dictionary of sport psychology: sport, exercise, and performing arts (pp. 53). London, UK: Academic Press/Elsevier.
  6. Lippke, S., & Cihlar, V. (2020). Social Participation during the Transition to Retirement: Findings on Work, Health and Physical Activity beyond Retirement from an Interview Study over the Course of 3 Years. Activities, Adaptation & Aging, 1-24.
  7. Lippke, S., & Schüz, B. (2019). Modelle gesundheitsbezogenen Handelns und Verhaltensänderung. In Gesundheitswissenschaften (pp. 299-310). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.
  8. Storm, V., Reinwand, D., Wienert, J., Kuhlmann, T., De Vries, H., & Lippke, S. (2017). Brief report: Compensatory health beliefs are negatively associated with intentions for regular fruit and vegetable consumption when self-efficacy is low. Journal of health psychology, 22(8), 1094-1100.
  9. Tan, S. L., Storm, V., Reinwand, D. A., Wienert, J., de Vries, H., & Lippke, S. (2018). Understanding the positive associations of sleep, physical activity, fruit and vegetable intake as predictors of quality of life and subjective health across age groups: a theory based, cross-sectional web-based study. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 977.
  10. Tan, S. L., Whittal, A., & Lippke, S. (2018). Associations among Sleep, Diet, Quality of Life, and Subjective Health. Health Behavior and Policy Review, 5(2), 46-58.
ScholarVision Creations