Children Resist Repeated Leading Questions and Social Pressures: Comparison
Please note this is a comparison between Version 2 by Catherine Yang and Version 1 by monia vagni.

In forensic contexts, children who are victims or witnesses of crimes are repeatedly questioned using stressful leading questions and social pressure. Younger children maintain a stable suggestive vulnerability and constant use of the same strategies to cope with cognitive and social risk factors of interrogative suggestibility, while older children could reduce their levels of yield and use more resistant responses that defer to greater source monitoring and less adherence to external expectations. Children, when exposed to repeated suggestive interviews, may learn to cope with more cognitive aspects of misleading questions while being less able to handle social–emotional pressures.

  • suggestibility
  • misleading information
  • criticism
  • resistant behavioural responses

1. Introduction

Particularly in forensic contexts, children who are victims or witnesses of crimes are heard as witnesses. Judicial practice highlights how they are often heard multiple times by police, professional figures, and judges. If children are already exposed to several suggestive interviews before giving their official testimony in court, it becomes important to clarify what effect repeated exposure to suggestive questions has, not only in terms of altering recollection but especially in terms of vulnerability.
The main aims of the present study are to verify whether repeated suggestive interviews lead to an increase or decrease in children’s level of suggestibility and resistant responses (Gudjonsson et al. 2021) and how age and intelligence quotient can reduce their vulnerability.
Several studies have shown how exposure to misinformation and leading questions can lead to memory impairment (Loftus 1979; Loftus et al. 1990; Lamb et al. 2008). Furthermore, the literature highlights that suggestibility is one of the principal risk factors for eyewitnesses and that immediate and delayed suggestibility are different and independent: the first concerns the way in which the individual answers the leading questions, while the second represents the tendency to incorporate misleading information into the original memory (Vagni et al. 2015; Gabbert and Hope 2018; Gudjonsson 2018).
Lamb and colleagues (Lamb et al. 2008) have shown that 80% of the questions that are addressed to children during forensic hearings are closed and require dichotomous yes/no or more alternative answers. The children’s ability to distinguish different responses and, above all, their effectiveness in rejecting both immediately—in the case of exposure to factors of repetition of suggestive questions and factors of emotional pressure, and, after time, the suggestions to which they may be exposed—represents a critical aspect of their accuracy and testimonial reliability.
According to several authors, children have less ability to source monitoring and trust in their own memory, leading to the risk of them confusing information suggestively provided and actually experienced, altering their original memory (Nelson and Fivush 2004; Bauer and Larkina 2016; Jack et al. 2016). Moreover, children in the forensic context are more affected by expectations of success, interpersonal trust, and authority of figures such as policemen or judges and are more vulnerable to emotional and social pressures (Vagni et al. 2018). It seems important to understand if and how these aspects can reduce the reliability of children’s memories when they are victims and witnesses of some crimes.
Few studies, however, seem to have considered what effect this produces in children in terms of their ability to respond to subsequent suggestive questions (Melnyk and Bruck 2004).
To better understand what the effects of repeated exposure to suggestive questions may be, it is first necessary to explain what is meant by interrogative suggestibility and resistant responses.

2. Interrogative Suggestibility

The paradigm of interrogative suggestibility (IS), also called immediate suggestibility, developed by Gudjonsson and Clark (1986), is a psychosocial model that refers to the social pressure that an interviewee receives during the process of answering suggestive questions.
The model of Gudjonsson and Clark (1986), which is used primarily in the forensic context, includes two distinctive and independent aspects of interrogative suggestibility: the impact of “leading questions” and “negative feedback” (Gudjonsson 2003). The “leading questions” are questions that contain suggestions to influence the response of the interviewee or witness through post-event information or misleading information. The negative feedback is a criticism directed at respondents to increase their degree of uncertainty and insecurity.
The main assumptions of IS are uncertainty, interpersonal trust, and expectations of success. Leading questions include misleading information, and this leads people to be uncertain about the correct answer. This occurs all the more when the suggestive information appears pertinent, plausible, and compatible with one’s knowledge and expectations. Interpersonal trust means that the witness believes that the interviewer’s intentions are true. Expectations of success indicate the belief of the witness that they are able to answer the questions correctly.
According to the IS model, there are two factors of suggestibility: yielding, which is the tendency to accept leading questions (Yield), and Shift, which is the tendency to change one’s answers following negative feedback (Gudjonsson 1984, 1987).
Previous studies indicated a strong negative relationship between intellectual ability and interrogative suggestibility (Gudjonsson 1983, 1990, 2018), and Frumkin et al. (2012) found intellectual ability to be more closely related to the tendency to yield to misleading questions than the tendency to shift answers following negative feedback.
In addition, regarding cognitive factors, age influences the level of suggestibility, given its relationship with developmental processes (Caso et al. 2013; Goodman et al. 2014). Several studies have shown that younger children are generally significantly more vulnerable to misleading questions (Ceci et al. 2007; Goodman et al. 2014), and this may be due to their poorer memory traces (Ceci 1994; Goodman 1984), lower language skills and less developed cognitive abilities than older children (Eysenck 2015).
Furthermore, according to Ceci and Bruck (2006), younger children are probably more influenced by social pressure and lack of social support. In relation to immediate suggestibility, age seems to have an impact on up to 12 years of age; subsequently, the performance of children over the age of 12 is similar to that of adults (Gudjonsson 2003).
Age appears to be a significant predictor of immediate suggestibility but not of delayed suggestibility (Lee 2004).
With regard to socioemotional factors, exposure to adverse life events, including being involved in court cases as witness, appears to correlate with higher levels of suggestibility (Drake 2010, 2011, 2014; Drake et al. 2008; Gudjonsson et al. 2020; Vagni et al. 2021), and reduces the ability to cope with interrogative pressure (shift) and repeated questioning (yield 2) (Drake 2014; Vagni et al. 2018, 2021).
Recent studies (Gudjonsson et al. 2021, 2022) highlighted the importance of also considering how children respond to suggestive questions and what resistant responses they can express.

3. Resistant Behavioural Responses

People have several ways to answer leading questions: accepting, rejecting, or admitting they do not know the answer. The different responses of refusal of a suggestion represent the Resistant Behavioural Responses.
The Resistant Behavioural Responses (RBR) is a model validated by (Gudjonsson et al. 2021, 2022) and is based on a source monitoring framework (SMF; Johnson 1997; Johnson et al. 1993; Johnson and Raye 1981) that “refers to a set of processes involved in making attributions about the origin of memory, knowledge, and beliefs” (Johnson et al. 1993, p. 3). People can answer leading questions by accepting, refusing with a simple “No” (NO answers), or expressing one’s uncertainty (“Don’t know”, DK answers). People who have high source monitoring of information respond by providing direct E\explanations (DE answers) and not just saying no. Many studies are concerned with “Don’t Know”-type answers (DK), such as RBR in children (e.g., Brubacher et al. 2015; Earhart et al. 2014; McWilliams et al. 2014; Waterman and Blades 2011), highlighting how younger children have difficult answering “don’t know” and declaring their uncertainty, probably due to their poorer understanding of unanswerable questions and greater expectation that they must provide either ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answers (Ceci and Bruck 1993).
A recent study on 360 children aged between 7 and 17 years (Gudjonsson et al. 2022) found that NO, DE, and DK answers are different and independent response styles that have different effects on resistance to misleading questions. In particular, “Direct Explanation” answers are the most stable and robust and increase incrementally with age in children (Gudjonsson et al. 2021). The results showed that DE answers are driven by different cognitive and social processes than DK and NO answers. The main difference between DE, DK, and NO answers is that for DE answers, people recognize the discrepancy between what was observed, showing effective strategic source monitoring and control processes.
For the purposes of the study, it is important to detect what the effects of repeated suggestive questions could be in terms of the degree of vulnerability and the ability to resist suggestibility.

4. Repeated Suggestive Interviewing

The effect of repeated suggestive questions over time has been investigated by other previous research, sometimes leading to conflicting results. Some studies found that repeated interviews can help support accurate memory (Goodman and Quas 2008; Hershkowitz et al. 2021) and may be helpful for building rapport with children (Faller 2014). However, excessive interviewing of children using suggestive techniques can be detrimental to accuracy (e.g., Ceci et al. 2007), especially if memory is weak for what occurred (Goodman and Quas 2008). Children are more likely to change their answers (e.g., to “I don’t know”) to repeated yes/no, forced-choice, or suggestive questions, or to challenges to their original answers (Candel et al. 2000).
Warren and Lane (1995) showed that the repetition of more suggestive interviews did not necessarily lead children to be more suggestible. Cassel et al. (1996) found that the tendency to yield to repeated-ended suggestive questions decreased with age leading 12-year-olds to perform similarly to adults and display stable levels of suggestibility (La Rooy et al. 2009). In answering repeated leading questions, various factors intervene, such as age (Gudjonsson 2003; Gudjonsson et al. 2016); higher source monitoring skills (Lee and Shin 2022); coping strategies (Maiorano and Vagni 2020; Rossi-Arnaud et al. 2023); language skills; stressful nature of the context, such as the forensic one (Vagni et al. 2018); and resilience skills (Gudjonsson et al. 2020; Gudjonsson et al. 2021). However, inter-subject variability in levels of vulnerability depends on individual differences (Klemfuss and Olaguez 2020).
With increasing age, children develop greater cognitive skills that allow them to better cope with the repetition of suggestive questions and the possible confusion effect deriving from misleading information and perceived social pressure during the interview.
According to Memon and Vartoukian (1996), children who were able to answer “Don’t know” and understood that this answer was plausible and accepted by the interviewer in subsequent interviews were more resistant.
Learning that a resistant answer is effective may lead children to use it more in subsequent interviews. People who have certain skills, such as memory confidence, metacognitive skills, ability to monitor sources, and critical analysis skills, are less suggestible and less influenced by context (Johnson and Raye 1981; Parker and Fischhoff 2005; Singh and Gudjonsson 1992). Often, these skills are not present in younger children because they require greater cognitive maturity.
Previous studies have verified the effect of repetitions of misleading questions using the same questions at a distance of time or creating similar interviews. Few studies have used the repetition of two parallel scales with the same psychometric properties that guarantee measurable and comparable results (Baxter and Bain 2002). The existing literature lacks a study that has measured what children’s effective resistance capacities may be when, over time, they are subjected to multiple suggestive interviews presenting misleading information. Indeed, no study has associated the effect of repetition of suggestive interviews in children with the Resistant Behavioral Responses model to test whether capacities of resistance vary or remain stable.

References

  1. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes, Monia Vagni, Tiziana Maiorano, Valeria Giostra, and Daniela Pajardi. 2021. Trauma symptoms of sexual abuse reduce resilience in children to give ‘no’ replies to misleading questions. Personality and Individual Differences 168: 110378.
  2. Loftus, Elizabeth F. 1979. Eyewitness Testimony. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  3. Loftus, Elizabeth F., Edith L. Greene, and James M. Doyle. 1990. The psychology of eyewitness testimony. In Psychological Methods in Criminal Investigations and Evidence. Edited by David C. Raskin. New York: Springer Publishing Company, pp. 34–35.
  4. Lamb, Michael E., Irit Hershkowitz, Yael Orbach, and Phillip W. Esplin. 2008. Tell Me What Happened: Structured Investigative Interviews of Child Victims and Witnesses. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
  5. Vagni, Monia, Tiziana Maiorano, Daniela Pajardi, and Gisli Hannes Gudjonsson. 2015. Immediate and delayed suggestibility among suspected child victims of sexual abuse. Personality and Individual Dfferences 79: 129–33.
  6. Gabbert, Fiona, and Lorraine Hope. 2018. Suggestibility in the courtroom: How memory can be distorted during the investigative and legal process. In Finding the Truth in the Courtroom: Dealing with Deception, Lies, and Memories. Edited by Henry Otgaar and Mark L. Howe. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 31–57.
  7. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes. 2018. The Psychology of False Confessions. Forty Years of Science and Practice. Chichester: Wiley Black.
  8. Nelson, Katherine, and Robyn Fivush. 2004. The Emergence of Autobiographical Memory: A Social Cultural Developmental Theory. Psychological Review 111: 486–511.
  9. Bauer, Patricia J., and Marina Larkina. 2016. Predicting remembering and forgetting of autobiographical memories in children and adults: A 4-year prospective study. Memory 24: 1345–68.
  10. Jack, Fiona, William Friedman, Elaine Reese, and Rachel Zajac. 2016. Age-related differences in memory for time, temporal reconstruction, and the availability and use of temporal landmarks. Cognitive Development 37: 53–66.
  11. Vagni, Monia, Tiziana Maiorano, Daniela Pajardi, and Manuela Berlingeri. 2018. Suggestionabilità interrogativa: Il ruolo del contesto forense e dello stress post traumatico in bambini e adolescenti testimoni di presunta violenza sessuale. Psicologia Sociale 2: 107–28.
  12. Melnyk, Laura, and Maggie Bruck. 2004. Timing moderates the effects of repeated suggestive interviewing on children’s eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 18: 613–31.
  13. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes, and Noel K. Clark. 1986. Suggestibility in police interrogation: A social psychological model. Social Behaviour 1: 83–104.
  14. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes. 2003. The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions. A Handbook. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
  15. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes. 1984. A new scale of interrogative suggestibility. Personality and Individual Differences 5: 303–14.
  16. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes. 1987. A parallel form of the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 26: 215–21.
  17. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes. 1983. Suggestibility, intelligence, memory recall and personality: An experimental study. The British Journal of Psychiatry 142: 35–37.
  18. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes. 1990. The relationship of intellectual skills to suggestibility, compliance and acquiescence. Personality and Individual Differences 11: 227–31.
  19. Frumkin, I. Bruce, Stephen J. Lally, and James E. Sexton. 2012. A United States forensic sample for the Gudjonsson suggestibility scales. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 30: 749–63.
  20. Caso, Letizia, Federica Soardi, and Franziska Paccanelli. 2013. La suggestionabilità interrogativa nei bambini: Una ricerca sperimentale sull’influenza dell’età e dell’autorevolezza dell’intervistatore. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia 40: 313–30.
  21. Goodman, Gail S., Christin M. Ogle, Kelly McWilliams, Rachel K. Narr, and Pedro M. Paz-Alonso. 2014. Memory Development in the Forensic Context. In The Wiley Handbook on the Development of Children’s Memory, 1st ed. Edited by Patricia J. Bauer and Robyn Fivush. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 921–61.
  22. Ceci, Stephen J., Paul B. Papierno, and Sarah Kulkofsky. 2007. Representational constraints on children’s suggestibility. Psychological Science 18: 503–9.
  23. Ceci, Stephen J. 1994. Cognitive and social factors in children’s testimony. In Psychology in Litigation and Legislation. Edited by Bruce D. Sales and Gary R. VandenBos. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 11–54.
  24. Goodman, Gail S. 1984. Children’s testimony in historical perspective. Journal of Social Issues 40: 9–31.
  25. Eysenck, Michael W. 2015. Memory in childhood. In Memory, 2nd ed. Edited by Alan D. Baddeley, Michael W. Eysenck and Michael C. Anderson. New York: Psychology Press, pp. 381–409.
  26. Ceci, Stephen J., and Maggie Bruck. 2006. Children’s suggestibility: Characteristics and mechanisms. Advances in Child Development and Behavior 34: 247–81.
  27. Lee, Kerry. 2004. Age, neuropsychological, and social cognitive measures as predictors of individual differences in susceptibility to the misinformation effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology 18: 997–1019.
  28. Drake, Kim E. 2010. Interrogative suggestibility: Life adversity, neuroticism and compliance. Personality and Individual Differences 48: 493–98.
  29. Drake, Kim E. 2011. Further insights into the relationship between the experience of life adversity and interrogative suggestibility. Personality and Individual Differences 51: 1056–58.
  30. Drake, Kim E. 2014. The role of trait anxiety in the association between the reporting of negative life events and interrogative suggestibility. Personality and Individidual Differences 60: 54–59.
  31. Drake, Kim E., Ray Bull, and Julian C. W. Boon. 2008. Interrogative suggestibility, self-esteem, and the influence of negative life events. Legal and Criminological Psychology 13: 299–307.
  32. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes, Monia Vagni, Tiziana Maiorano, and Daniela Pajardi. 2020. The relationship between trauma symptoms and immediate and delayed suggestibility in children who have been sexually abused. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling 17: 1–14.
  33. Vagni, Monia, Tiziana Maiorano, and Valeria Giostra. 2021. The relationship between suggestibility, fabrication, distortion, and trauma in suspected sexually abused children. Social Sciences 10: 37.
  34. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes, Monia Vagni, Tiziana Maiorano, Valeria Giostra, and Daniela Pajardi. 2022. The relative impact of different ‘resistant behavioural responses’ on interrogative suggestibility in children: The powerful contribution of ‘direct explanation’replies to unanswerable questions. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling 19: 3–19.
  35. Johnson, Marcia K. 1997. Source monitoring and memory distortion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Series B: Biological Sciences 352: 1733–45.
  36. Johnson, Marcia K., Shahin Hashtroudi, and D. Stephen Lindsay. 1993. Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin 114: 3.
  37. Johnson, Marcia K., and Carol L. Raye. 1981. Reality monitoring. Psychological Review 88: 67.
  38. Brubacher, Sonja P., Debra Ann Poole, and Jason J. Dickinson. 2015. The use of ground rules in investigative interviews with children: A synthesis and call for research. Developmental Review 36: 15–33.
  39. Earhart, Becky, David J. La Rooy, Sonja P. Brubacher, and Michael E. Lamb. 2014. An examination of “don’t know” responses in forensic interviews with children. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 32: 746–61.
  40. McWilliams, Kelly, Latonya S. Harris, and Gail S. Goodman. 2014. Child maltreatment, trauma-related psychopathology, and eyewitness memory in children and adolescents. Behavioral Science and Law 32: 702–17.
  41. Waterman, Amanda H., and Mark Blades. 2011. Helping children correctly say “I don’t know” to unanswerable questions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 17: 396.
  42. Ceci, Stephen J., and Maggie Bruck. 1993. Suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin 113: 403.
  43. Goodman, Gail S., and Jodi A. Quas. 2008. Repeated interviews and children’s memory: It’s more than just how many. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17: 386–90.
  44. Hershkowitz, Irit, Michael E. Lamb, Uri Blasbalg, and Yael Karni-Visel. 2021. The dynamics of two-session interviews with suspected victims of abuse who are reluctant to make allegations. Development and Psychopathology 33: 739–47.
  45. Faller, Kathleen Coulborn. 2014. Forty years of forensic interviewing of children suspected of sexual abuse, 1974–2014: Historical benchmarks. Social Sciences 4: 34–65.
  46. Candel, Ingrid, Harald Merckelbach, and Peter Muris. 2000. Measuring interrogative suggestibility in children: Reliability and validity of the Bonn Test of Statement Suggestibility. Psychology, Crime & Law 6: 61–70.
  47. Warren, Amye R., and Peggy Lane. 1995. Effects of timing and type of questioning on eyewitness accuracy and suggestibility. In Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness. Edited by Maria S. Zaragoza, John R. Graham, Gordon C. N. Hall, Richard Hirschman and Yossef S. Ben-Porath. New York: Sage Publications, pp. 44–60.
  48. Cassel, William S., Claudia E. M. Roebers, and David F. Bjorklund. 1996. Developmental patterns of eyewitness responses to repeated and increasingly suggestive questions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 61: 116–33.
  49. La Rooy, David, Michael E. Lamb, and Margaret-Ellen Pipe. 2009. Repeated interviewing: A critical evaluation of the risks and potential benefits. In Child Sexual Abuse: Research, Evaluation, and Testimony for the Courts. Edited by K. Kuehnle and M. Connell. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 327–61.
  50. Gudjonsson, Gisli Hannes, Monia Vagni, Tiziana Maiorano, and Daniela Pajardi. 2016. Age and memory related changes in children’s immediate and delayed suggestibility using the Gudjonsson suggestibility scale. Personality and Individual Differences 102: 25–29.
  51. Lee, Seungjin, and Minkyeong Shin. 2022. An overview of source monitoring theory and research regarding children’s training. Current Psychology, 1–16.
  52. Maiorano, Tiziana, and Monia Vagni. 2020. Coping Strategies, Immediate and Delayed Suggestibility among Children and Adolescents. Social Sciences 9: 186.
  53. Rossi-Arnaud, Clelia, Pietro Spataro, Serena Mastroberardino, Enrica Lucaroni, Anna Maria Giannini, and Vincenzo Cestari. 2023. Why collaboration reduces suggestibility: The role of source-monitoring processes and retrieval strategies. Current Psychology 42: 6386–94.
  54. Klemfuss, J. Zoe, and Alma P. Olaguez. 2020. Individual differences in children’s suggestibility: An updated review. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse 29: 158–82.
  55. Memon, Amina, and Rita Vartoukian. 1996. The effects of repeated questioning on young children’s eyewitness testimony. British Journal of Psychology 87: 403–15.
  56. Parker, Andrew M., and Baruch Fischhoff. 2005. Decision-making competence: External validation through an individual-differences approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18: 1–27.
  57. Singh, Krishna K., and Gisli H. Gudjonsson. 1992. Interrogative suggestibility among adolescent boys and its relationship with intelligence, memory, and cognitive set. Journal of Adolescence 15: 155–61.
  58. Baxter, James S., and Stella A. Bain. 2002. Faking interrogative suggestiblity: The truth machine. Legal and Criminological Psychology 7: 219–25.
More
Video Production Service