Submitted Successfully!
To reward your contribution, here is a gift for you: A free trial for our video production service.
Thank you for your contribution! You can also upload a video entry or images related to this topic.
Version Summary Created by Modification Content Size Created at Operation
1 + 900 word(s) 900 2021-06-17 07:55:55

Video Upload Options

Do you have a full video?

Confirm

Are you sure to Delete?
Cite
If you have any further questions, please contact Encyclopedia Editorial Office.
Meyer, K.; Sommer, W.; Hildebrandt, A. Face Cognition. Encyclopedia. Available online: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/11601 (accessed on 29 March 2024).
Meyer K, Sommer W, Hildebrandt A. Face Cognition. Encyclopedia. Available at: https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/11601. Accessed March 29, 2024.
Meyer, Kristina, Werner Sommer, Andrea Hildebrandt. "Face Cognition" Encyclopedia, https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/11601 (accessed March 29, 2024).
Meyer, K., Sommer, W., & Hildebrandt, A. (2021, July 02). Face Cognition. In Encyclopedia. https://encyclopedia.pub/entry/11601
Meyer, Kristina, et al. "Face Cognition." Encyclopedia. Web. 02 July, 2021.
Face Cognition
Edit

The term Face Cognition (FC) describes a set of specific abilities related to the processing of faces, including face perception and face memory.

socio-cognitive abilities face cognition face specificity speed and accuracy

1. Introduction

The study of between-person differences in cognitive abilities is concerned with questions such as: what is the structure of cognitive abilities? Which higher- and lower-order abilities can be identified? Further, which abilities are specific, i.e. which are differentiated from other abilities, and which abilities overlap to such a high extent with others that they cannot be distinguished?

Faces are a source of information of paramount importance to social interaction from an evolutionary viewpoint [1]. Therefore, it is not surprising that specificity of face cognition (FC) has been widely accepted [2][3], but it’s origins remain a matter of debate. Some researchers propose that FC is a specific ability due to properties such as heritability, specialized neural structures involved in FC and the possibility of isolated FC impairment in the sense of double dissociation from other visual object processing abilities [4][5][6][7]. In contrast, proponents of the expertise hypothesis hold that faces are objects all adults have grown up to be experts with, and that expertise is the true driving force giving rise to any object processing specificity [8][9]

In individual differences research, specificity of an ability describes the occurrence of a pattern of between-person correlations in task performance. If performance scores in different cognitive tasks are highly correlated, it suggests a common underlying cause. Performance in various face tasks correlate more with each other than with performance in non-face tasks. Such between-person variance patterns can be investigated using structural equation modeling (SEM).

2. On Accuracy- and Speed-Related Face Cognition Abilities

Cognitive ability research encompasses performance measures from difficult and easy tasks. Accuracy is typically measured in difficult tasks and operationalized as the proportion of responses. Easy tasks, on the other hand, are usually solved correctly by most individuals; thus, accuracy is usually at ceiling and response times (RTs) are investigated instead.

Wilhelm and colleagues [10] first discovered a differentiation in the ability structure of accuracy- versus speed-based FC measures acquired from difficult and easy FC tasks, respectively. The authors asked their participants to solve tasks of face perception and face memory tasks in easy and difficult conditions and investigated the underlying ability structure using SEMs. While accuracy measures were specific (i.e., individuals could be accurate in difficult face memory tasks but inaccurate in difficult face perception tasks), performance speed in easy tasks was strongly related and indistinguishable across all tasks).

This surprising finding of a differentiated ability structure in difficult tasks and a unitary ability structure in speed was later extended. For example, the factor structure of accuracy tasks was reported to be stable across the adult life span [11] and across sexes [12].

Whereas the differentiated structure of FC accuracy in difficult tasks seems to be a clear and robust finding across studies, waters are muddier in the case of FC speed. Some studies found face specificity in speed tasks, some studies reported a unitary factor of performance speed like Wilhelm and colleagues [10].

3. Conditions of Face Specificity

To carve out which task properties result in face specificity in easy tasks and which do not, Meyer et al. [13] compared task characteristics across studies. The authors find that the difference lies in whether SEMs are based on performance in several trials of the same task, or whether SEMs are based on different tasks. In studies reporting no specificity for faces in RTs, multiple tasks had been employed. For example, Nowparast Rostami et al. [14] modelled speed across a battery of face and house perception and memory tasks. In studies finding face specificity in RTs on easy tasks, performance was modelled within the same or similar tasks. For example, Ćepulić et al. [15] used the same kind of recognition task but varied the number of stimuli to be remembered and the content of the stimuli (faces vs. different kinds of objects).

Meyer et al. [13] explain the divergence of results as follows: If multiple tasks are jointly modelled, the latent factors of the SEMs represent common factors underlying all the tasks. Thus, in difficult FC tasks, a common face perception or face memory ability is employed. In easy tasks, however, there clearly seems to be face-specific variance, but it seems to be also task-specific. Thus, whereas there is a common underlying FC-related ability influencing performance in various difficult tasks, the underlying FC-related abilities in easy tasks are less associated with each other. In other words, the face-specific speed abilities show less overlap. Therefore, there is face specificity in easy tasks, but it is only visible if the task performance is modelled alone. According to the authors, this explains why studies find face-specific factors of speed in single easy tasks, but not when jointly modelling multiple easy tasks (see Figure 1).

Jintelligence 09 00030 g003 550

Figure 1. Face specificity in single vs. multiple FC tasks of increasing difficulty. Circular shapes represent underlying abilities (constructs) while angular shapes represent observed performance scores. G—general factor; F—face-specific factor. Task 1–3—these represent possible tasks as proposed in FC research, such as tasks constructed by Herzmann et al. [18] and applied by Wilhelm et al. [10]. These tasks are not all on the same difficulty level; rather, the tasks position on the easy–hard continuum influences the ratio of task-specific vs. task-independent face processing ability it taps into, represented as face-specific factors in the figure. Figure taken from Meyer et al. [13]

References

  1. Robin I. M. Dunbar; The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 1998, 6, 178-190, 10.1002/(sici)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::aid-evan5>3.0.co;2-8.
  2. Galit Yovel; Jeremy B. Wilmer; Brad Duchaine; What can individual differences reveal about face processing?. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2014, 8, 562, 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00562.
  3. Nancy Kanwisher; Domain specificity in face perception. Nature Neuroscience 2000, 3, 759-763, 10.1038/77664.
  4. Joachim Bodamer; Die Prosop-Agnosie. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 1947, 179, 6-53, 10.1007/bf00352849.
  5. Nancy Kanwisher; Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2010, 107, 11163-11170, 10.1073/pnas.1005062107.
  6. Jürgen M. Kaufmann; Stefan Schweinberger; Anthony Michael Burton; N250 ERP Correlates of the Acquisition of Face Representations across Different Images. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2009, 21, 625-641, 10.1162/jocn.2009.21080.
  7. Jeremy B. Wilmer; Laura Germine; Christopher F. Chabris; Garga Chatterjee; Mark Williams; Eric Loken; Ken Nakayama; Bradley Duchaine; Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2010, 107, 5238-5241, 10.1073/pnas.0913053107.
  8. Cindy M. Bukach; Isabel Gauthier; Michael J. Tarr; Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2006, 10, 159-166, 10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.004.
  9. I. Gauthier; R. McGugin; J. Richler; G. Herzmann; M. Speegle; A. Van Gulick; Experience with objects moderates the overlap between object and face recognition performance, suggesting a common ability. Journal of Vision 2013, 13, 982-982, 10.1167/13.9.982.
  10. Oliver Wilhelm; Grit Herzmann; Olga Kunina-Habenicht; Vanessa Danthiir; Annekathrin Schacht; Werner Sommer; Individual differences in perceiving and recognizing faces—One element of social cognition.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2010, 99, 530-548, 10.1037/a0019972.
  11. Andrea Hildebrandt; Oliver Wilhelm; Grit Herzmann; Werner Sommer; Face and object cognition across adult age.. Psychology and Aging 2013, 28, 243-248, 10.1037/a0031490.
  12. Werner Sommer; Andrea Hildebrandt; Olga Kunina-Habenicht; Annekathrin Schacht; Oliver Wilhelm; Sex differences in face cognition. Acta Psychologica 2013, 142, 62-73, 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.11.001.
  13. Kristina Meyer; Werner Sommer; Andrea Hildebrandt; Reflections and New Perspectives on Face Cognition as a Specific Socio-Cognitive Ability. Journal of Intelligence 2021, 9, 30, 10.3390/jintelligence9020030.
  14. Hadiseh Nowparast Rostami; Werner Sommer; Changsong Zhou; Oliver Wilhelm; Andrea Hildebrandt; Structural encoding processes contribute to individual differences in face and object cognition: Inferences from psychometric test performance and event-related brain potentials. Cortex 2017, 95, 192-210, 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.017.
  15. Dominik-Borna Ćepulić; Oliver Wilhelm; Werner Sommer; Andrea Hildebrandt; All categories are equal, but some categories are more equal than others: The psychometric structure of object and face cognition.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2018, 44, 1254-1268, 10.1037/xlm0000511.
  16. Kristina Meyer; Hadiseh Nowparast Rostami; Guang Ouyang; Stefan Debener; Werner Sommer; Andrea Hildebrandt; Mechanisms of Face Specificity – Differentiating Speed and Accuracy in Face Cognition by Event-Related Potentials of Central Processing. Cortex 2021, 134, 114-133, 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.10.016.
  17. Guang Ouyang; Andrea Hildebrandt; Florian Schmitz; Christoph S. Herrmann; Decomposing alpha and 1/f brain activities reveals their differential associations with cognitive processing speed. NeuroImage 2020, 205, 116304, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116304.
  18. Grit Herzmann; Vanessa Danthiir; Annekathrin Schacht; Werner Sommer; Oliver Wilhelm; Toward a comprehensive test battery for face cognition: Assessment of the tasks. Behavior Research Methods 2008, 40, 840-857, 10.3758/brm.40.3.840.
More
Information
Subjects: Psychology
Contributors MDPI registered users' name will be linked to their SciProfiles pages. To register with us, please refer to https://encyclopedia.pub/register : , ,
View Times: 564
Revision: 1 time (View History)
Update Date: 02 Jul 2021
1000/1000